106 research outputs found

    Heuristics for Broader Assessment of Effectiveness and Usability in Technology-Mediated Technical Communication

    Get PDF
    Purpose: To offer additional tools for the assessment of effectiveness and usability in technology-mediated communication based in established heuristics. Method: An interdisciplinary group of researchers at Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute selected five disparate examples of technology-mediated communication, formally evaluated each using contemporary heuristics, and then engaged in an iterative design process to arrive at an expanded toolkit for in depth analyses. Results: A set of heuristics and operationalized metrics for the deeper analysis of a broader scope of contemporary technology-mediated communication. Conclusions: The continual evolution of communication, including the emergence of new, interactive media, provides a challenging opportunity to identify effective approaches and techniques. There are benefits to a renewed focus on relationships between people and between people and information, and we offer additional criteria and metrics to supplement established means of heuristic analysis

    Mitochondrial Protein Lipoylation and the 2-Oxoglutarate Dehydrogenase Complex Controls HIF1α Stability in Aerobic Conditions.

    Get PDF
    Hypoxia-inducible transcription factors (HIFs) control adaptation to low oxygen environments by activating genes involved in metabolism, angiogenesis, and redox homeostasis. The finding that HIFs are also regulated by small molecule metabolites highlights the need to understand the complexity of their cellular regulation. Here we use a forward genetic screen in near-haploid human cells to identify genes that stabilize HIFs under aerobic conditions. We identify two mitochondrial genes, oxoglutarate dehydrogenase (OGDH) and lipoic acid synthase (LIAS), which when mutated stabilize HIF1α in a non-hydroxylated form. Disruption of OGDH complex activity in OGDH or LIAS mutants promotes L-2-hydroxyglutarate formation, which inhibits the activity of the HIFα prolyl hydroxylases (PHDs) and TET 2-oxoglutarate dependent dioxygenases. We also find that PHD activity is decreased in patients with homozygous germline mutations in lipoic acid synthesis, leading to HIF1 activation. Thus, mutations affecting OGDHC activity may have broad implications for epigenetic regulation and tumorigenesis.This work was supported by a Wellcome Trust Senior Clinical Research Fellowship to J.A.N. (102770/Z/13/Z), Wellcome Trust Principal Research Fellowship to P.J.L. (084957/Z/08/Z), and the Medical Research Council (A.S.H.C. and C.F.). The Cambridge Institute for Medical Research is in receipt of a Wellcome Trust Strategic Award (100140).This is the final version of the article. It first appeared from Elsevier (Cell Press) via https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmet.2016.09.01

    Integrated stratigraphic correlation of Upper Devonian platform-to-basin carbonate sequences, Lennard Shelf, Canning Basin, Western Australia: advances in carbonate margin-to-slope sequence stratigraphy and stacking patterns

    Get PDF
    High-resolution, time-significant correlations are integral to meaningful stratigraphic frameworks in depositional systems, but may be difficult to achieve using traditional sequence stratigraphic or biostratigraphic approaches alone, particularly in geologically complex settings. In steep, reefal carbonate margin-to-slope systems, such correlations are essential to unravel shelf-to-basin transitions, characterize strike variability, and develop predictive sequence stratigraphic models – concepts which are currently poorly understood in these heterogeneous settings. The Canning Basin Chronostratigraphy Project (CBCP) integrates multiple independent datasets (including biostratigraphy, magnetostratigraphy, stable isotope chemostratigraphy, and sequence stratigraphy) extracted from Upper Devonian (Frasnian and Famennian) reefal platform exposures along the Lennard Shelf, Canning Basin, Western Australia. These were used to generate a well-constrained stratigraphic framework and shelf-to-basin composite reconstruction of the carbonate system. The resultant integrated framework allows for unprecedented analysis of carbonate margin-to-slope heterogeneity, depositional architecture, and sequence stratigraphy along the Lennard Shelf. Systems tract architecture, facies partitioning, and stacking patterns of margin to lower-slope environments were assessed for six composite-scale sequences that form part of a transgressive-to-regressive supersequence and span the Frasnian-Famennian (F-F) biotic crisis. Variations are apparent in margin styles, foreslope facies proportions, dominant resedimentation processes, downslope contributing sediment factories, and vertical rock successions, related to hierarchical accommodation signals and ecological changes associated with F-F boundary. We present these results in the form of carbonate margin-to-basin sequence stratigraphic models and associations that link seismic-scale architecture to fine-scale facies heterogeneity. These models provide a predictive foundation for characterization of steep-sided flanks of reefal carbonate platform systems that is useful for both industry and academia. This study emphasizes the utility of an integrated stratigraphic approach and the insights gained from better-constrained facies and stratal architecture analysis; insights that were not achievable with traditional sequence stratigraphic or biostratigraphic techniques alone

    Optimal Relevance in Imperfect Information Games

    Full text link
    To help incorporate natural language into economic theory, this paper does two things. First, the paper extends to imperfect information games an equilibrium concept developed for incomplete information games, so natural language can be formalized as a vehicle to convey information about actions as well as types. This equilibrium concept is specific to language games, because information is conveyed by the sender through the message's literal meaning. Second, the paper proposes an equilibrium refinement which selects the sender's most preferred equilibrium. The refinement captures the notion that the speaker seeks to improve its status quo, aiming at optimal relevance. Explicit coordination through verbal communication parallels the idea of implicit coordination through focal points

    How do violations of Gricean maxims affect reading?

    Get PDF
    Four eye-tracking experiments examined how violations of the Gricean maxim of quantity affect reading. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that first-pass reading times for size-modified definite nouns (the small towel) were longer when the modifier was redundant, as the context contained one rather than two possible referents, whereas first-pass times for bare nouns (the towel) were unaffected by whether the context contained multiple referents that resulted in ambiguity. Experiment 3 showed that unlike redundant size modifiers, redundant color modifiers did not increase first-pass times. Experiment 4 confirmed this finding, demonstrating that the effect of redundancy was dependent on the meaning of the modifier. We propose that initial referential processing is led by the lexico-semantic representation of the referring expression rather than Gricean expectations about optimal informativeness: Redundancy of a size-modifier immediately disrupts comprehension because the processor fails to activate the referential contrast implied by the meaning of the modifier, whereas referential ambiguity has no immediate effect, as it allows the activation of at least one semantically-compatible referent

    Irony Detection in Twitter: The Role of Affective Content

    Full text link
    © ACM 2016. This is the author's version of the work. It is posted here for your personal use. Not for redistribution. The definitive Version of Record was published in ACM Transactions on Internet Technology, Vol. 16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2930663.[EN] Irony has been proven to be pervasive in social media, posing a challenge to sentiment analysis systems. It is a creative linguistic phenomenon where affect-related aspects play a key role. In this work, we address the problem of detecting irony in tweets, casting it as a classification problem. We propose a novel model that explores the use of affective features based on a wide range of lexical resources available for English, reflecting different facets of affect. Classification experiments over different corpora show that affective information helps in distinguishing among ironic and nonironic tweets. Our model outperforms the state of the art in almost all cases.The National Council for Science and Technology (CONACyT Mexico) has funded the research work of Delia Irazu Hernandez Farias (Grant No. 218109/313683 CVU-369616). The work of Viviana Patti was partially carried out at the Universitat Politecnica de Valencia within the framework of a fellowship of the University of Turin cofunded by Fondazione CRT (World Wide Style Program 2). The work of Paolo Rosso has been partially funded by the SomEMBED TIN2015-71147-C2-1-P MINECO research project and by the Generalitat Valenciana under the grant ALMAMATER (PrometeoII/2014/030).Hernandez-Farias, DI.; Patti, V.; Rosso, P. (2016). Irony Detection in Twitter: The Role of Affective Content. ACM Transactions on Internet Technology. 16(3):19:1-19:24. https://doi.org/10.1145/2930663S19:119:24163Rob Abbott, Marilyn Walker, Pranav Anand, Jean E. Fox Tree, Robeson Bowmani, and Joseph King. 2011. How can you say such things?!?: Recognizing disagreement in informal political argument. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Languages in Social Media (LSM’11). Association for Computational Linguistics, Stroudsburg, PA, USA, 2--11.Laura Alba-Juez and Salvatore Attardo. 2014. The evaluative palette of verbal irony. In Evaluation in Context, Geoff Thompson and Laura Alba-Juez (Eds.). John Benjamins Publishing Company, Amsterdam/ Philadelphia, 93--116.Magda B. Arnold. 1960. Emotion and Personality. Vol. 1. Columbia University Press, New York, NY.Giuseppe Attardi, Valerio Basile, Cristina Bosco, Tommaso Caselli, Felice Dell’Orletta, Simonetta Montemagni, Viviana Patti, Maria Simi, and Rachele Sprugnoli. 2015. State of the art language technologies for italian: The EVALITA 2014 perspective. Journal of Intelligenza Artificiale 9, 1 (2015), 43--61.Salvatore Attardo. 2000. Irony as relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics 32, 6 (2000), 793--826.Stefano Baccianella, Andrea Esuli, and Fabrizio Sebastiani. 2010. SentiWordNet 3.0: An enhanced lexical resource for sentiment analysis and opinion mining. In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’10). European Language Resources Association (ELRA), Valletta, Malta, 2200,2204.David Bamman and Noah A. Smith. 2015. Contextualized sarcasm detection on twitter. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Web and Social Media, (ICWSM’15). AAAI, Oxford, UK, 574--577.Francesco Barbieri, Horacio Saggion, and Francesco Ronzano. 2014. Modelling sarcasm in twitter, a novel approach. In Proceedings of the 5th Workshop on Computational Approaches to Subjectivity, Sentiment and Social Media Analysis. Association for Computational Linguistics, Baltimore, Maryland, 50--58.Valerio Basile, Andrea Bolioli, Malvina Nissim, Viviana Patti, and Paolo Rosso. 2014. Overview of the evalita 2014 SENTIment POLarity classification task. In Proceedings of the 4th Evaluation Campaign of Natural Language Processing and Speech tools for Italian (EVALITA’14). Pisa University Press, Pisa, Italy, 50--57.Cristina Bosco, Viviana Patti, and Andrea Bolioli. 2013. Developing corpora for sentiment analysis: The case of irony and senti-TUT. IEEE Intelligent Systems 28, 2 (March 2013), 55--63.Andrea Bowes and Albert Katz. 2011. When sarcasm stings. Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal 48, 4 (2011), 215--236.Margaret M. Bradley and Peter J. Lang. 1999. Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Instruction Manual and Affective Ratings. Technical Report. Center for Research in Psychophysiology, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida.Konstantin Buschmeier, Philipp Cimiano, and Roman Klinger. 2014. An impact analysis of features in a classification approach to irony detection in product reviews. In Proceedings of the 5th Workshop on Computational Approaches to Subjectivity, Sentiment and Social Media Analysis. Association for Computational Linguistics, Baltimore, Maryland, 42--49.Erik Cambria, Andrew Livingstone, and Amir Hussain. 2012. The hourglass of emotions. In Cognitive Behavioural Systems. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 7403. Springer, Berlin, 144--157.Erik Cambria, Daniel Olsher, and Dheeraj Rajagopal. 2014. SenticNet 3: A common and common-sense knowledge base for cognition-driven sentiment analysis. In Proceedings of AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence. AAAI, Québec, Canada, 1515--1521.Jorge Carrillo de Albornoz, Laura Plaza, and Pablo Gervás. 2012. SentiSense: An easily scalable concept-based affective lexicon for sentiment analysis. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’12) (23-25), Nicoletta Calzolari (Conference Chair), Khalid Choukri, Thierry Declerck, Mehmet Ugur Dogan, Bente Maegaard, Joseph Mariani, Asuncion Moreno, Jan Odijk, and Stelios Piperidis (Eds.). European Language Resources Association (ELRA), Istanbul, Turkey, 3562--3567.Paula Carvalho, Luís Sarmento, Mário J. Silva, and Eugénio de Oliveira. 2009. Clues for detecting irony in user-generated contents: Oh&hallip;!! It’s “so easy” ;-). In Proceedings of the 1st International CIKM Workshop on Topic-sentiment Analysis for Mass Opinion (TSA’09). ACM, New York, NY, 53--56.Yoonjung Choi and Janyce Wiebe. 2014. +/-EffectWordNet: Sense-level lexicon acquisition for opinion inference. In Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP’14). Association for Computational Linguistics, Doha, Qatar, 1181--1191.Dmitry Davidov, Oren Tsur, and Ari Rappoport. 2010. Semi-supervised recognition of sarcastic sentences in twitter and amazon. In Proceedings of the 14th Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning (CoNLL’10). Association for Computational Linguistics, Uppsala, Sweden, 107--116.Shelly Dews, Joan Kaplan, and Ellen Winner. 1995. Why not say it directly? The social functions of irony. Discourse Processes 19, 3 (1995), 347--367.Paul Ekman. 1992. An argument for basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion 6, 3--4 (1992), 169--200.Elisabetta Fersini, Federico Alberto Pozzi, and Enza Messina. 2015. Detecting irony and sarcasm in microblogs: The role of expressive signals and ensemble classifiers. In 2015 IEEE International Conference on Data Science and Advanced Analytics (DSAA’15). IEEE Xplore Digital Library, Paris, France, 1--8.Elena Filatova. 2012. Irony and sarcasm: Corpus generation and analysis using crowdsourcing. In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’12). European Language Resources Association (ELRA), Istanbul, 392--398.Aniruddha Ghosh, Guofu Li, Tony Veale, Paolo Rosso, Ekaterina Shutova, John Barnden, and Antonio Reyes. 2015. SemEval-2015 task 11: Sentiment analysis of figurative language in twitter. In Proceedings of the 9th International Workshop on Semantic Evaluation (SemEval’15). Association for Computational Linguistics, Denver, Colorado, 470--478.Raymond W. Gibbs. 2000. Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and Symbol 15, 1--2 (2000), 5--27.Rachel Giora and Salvatore Attardo. 2014. Irony. In Encyclopedia of Humor Studies. SAGE, Thousand Oaks, CA.Rachel Giora and Ofer Fein. 1999. Irony: Context and salience. Metaphor and Symbol 14, 4 (1999), 241--257.Roberto González-Ibáñez, Smaranda Muresan, and Nina Wacholder. 2011. Identifying sarcasm in twitter: A closer look. In Proceedings of the 49th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies (HLT’11). Association for Computational Linguistics, Portland, OR, 581--586.H. Paul Grice. 1975. Logic and conversation. In Syntax and Semantics: Vol. 3: Speech Acts, P. Cole and J. L. Morgan (Eds.). Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 41--58.Irazú Hernández Farías, José-Miguel Benedí, and Paolo Rosso. 2015. Applying basic features from sentiment analysis for automatic irony detection. In Pattern Recognition and Image Analysis. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 9117. Springer International Publishing, Santiago de Compostela, Spain, 337--344.Minqing Hu and Bing Liu. 2004. Mining and summarizing customer reviews. In Proceedings of the 10th ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining (KDD’04). ACM, Seattle, WA, 168--177.Aditya Joshi, Vinita Sharma, and Pushpak Bhattacharyya. 2015. Harnessing context incongruity for sarcasm detection. In Proceedings of the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics and the 7th International Joint Conference on Natural Language Processing (Volume 2: Short Papers). Association for Computational Linguistics, Beijing, China, 757--762.Jihen Karoui, Farah Benamara, Véronique Moriceau, Nathalie Aussenac-Gilles, and Lamia Hadrich-Belguith. 2015. Towards a contextual pragmatic model to detect irony in tweets. In Proceedings of the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics and the 7th International Joint Conference on Natural Language Processing (Volume 2: Short Papers). Association for Computational Linguistics, Beijing, China, 644--650.Roger J. Kreuz and Gina M. Caucci. 2007. Lexical influences on the perception of sarcasm. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Computational Approaches to Figurative Language (FigLanguages’07). Association for Computational Linguistics, Rochester, NY, 1--4.Florian Kunneman, Christine Liebrecht, Margot van Mulken, and Antal van den Bosch. 2015. Signaling sarcasm: From hyperbole to hashtag. Information Processing & Management 51, 4 (2015), 500--509.Christopher Lee and Albert Katz. 1998. The differential role of ridicule in sarcasm and irony. Metaphor and Symbol 13, 1 (1998), 1--15.John S. Leggitt and Raymond W. Gibbs. 2000. Emotional reactions to verbal irony. Discourse Processes 29, 1 (2000), 1--24.Stephanie Lukin and Marilyn Walker. 2013. Really? Well. Apparently bootstrapping improves the performance of sarcasm and nastiness classifiers for online dialogue. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Language Analysis in Social Media. Association for Computational Linguistics, Atlanta, GA, 30--40.Diana Maynard and Mark Greenwood. 2014. Who cares about sarcastic tweets? Investigating the impact of sarcasm on sentiment analysis. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’14) (26-31). European Language Resources Association (ELRA), Reykjavik, Iceland, 4238--4243.Skye McDonald. 2007. Neuropsychological studies of sarcasm. In Irony in Language and Thought: A Cognitive Science Reader, H. Colston and R. Gibbs (Eds.). Lawrence Erlbaum, 217--230.Saif M. Mohammad and Peter D. Turney. 2013. Crowdsourcing a word--emotion association lexicon. Computational Intelligence 29, 3 (2013), 436--465.Saif M. Mohammad, Xiaodan Zhu, Svetlana Kiritchenko, and Joel Martin. 2015. Sentiment, emotion, purpose, and style in electoral tweets. Information Processing & Management 51, 4 (2015), 480--499.Finn Årup Nielsen. 2011. A new ANEW: Evaluation of a word list for sentiment analysis in microblogs. In Proceedings of the ESWC2011 Workshop on “Making Sense of Microposts”: Big Things Come in Small Packages (CEUR Workshop Proceedings), Vol. 718. CEUR-WS.org, Heraklion, Crete, Greece, 93--98.W. Gerrod Parrot. 2001. Emotions in Social Psychology: Essential Readings. Psychology Press, Philadelphia, PA.James W. Pennebaker, Martha E. Francis, and Roger J. Booth. 2001. Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count: LIWC 2001. Mahway: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 71.Robert Plutchik. 2001. The nature of emotions. American Scientist 89, 4 (2001), 344--350.Soujanya Poria, Alexander Gelbukh, Amir Hussain, Newton Howard, Dipankar Das, and Sivaji Bandyopadhyay. 2013. Enhanced senticnet with affective labels for concept-based opinion mining. IEEE Intelligent Systems 28, 2 (2013), 31--38.Tomáš Ptáček, Ivan Habernal, and Jun Hong. 2014. Sarcasm detection on Czech and English twitter. In Proceedings of the 25th International Conference on Computational Linguistics (COLING’14). Dublin City University and Association for Computational Linguistics, Dublin, Ireland, 213--223.Ashwin Rajadesingan, Reza Zafarani, and Huan Liu. 2015. Sarcasm detection on twitter: A behavioral modeling approach. In Proceedings of the 8th ACM International Conference on Web Search and Data Mining (WSDM’15). ACM, 97--106.Antonio Reyes and Paolo Rosso. 2014. On the difficulty of automatically detecting irony: Beyond a simple case of negation. Knowledge Information Systems 40, 3 (2014), 595--614.Antonio Reyes, Paolo Rosso, and Tony Veale. 2013. A multidimensional approach for detecting irony in twitter. Language Resources and Evaluation 47, 1 (2013), 239--268.Ellen Riloff, Ashequl Qadir, Prafulla Surve, Lalindra De Silva, Nathan Gilbert, and Ruihong Huang. 2013. Sarcasm as contrast between a positive sentiment and negative situation. In Proceedings of the 2013 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing, (EMNLP’13). Association for Computational Linguistics, Seattle, Washington, 704--714.Simone Shamay-Tsoory, Rachel Tomer, B. D. Berger, Dorith Goldsher, and Judith Aharon-Peretz. 2005. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cognitive Behavioral Neurology 18, 1 (2005), 55--67.Philip J. Stone and Earl B. Hunt. 1963. A computer approach to content analysis: Studies using the general inquirer system. In Proceedings of the May 21-23, 1963, Spring Joint Computer Conference (AFIPS’63 (Spring)). ACM, New York, NY, 241--256.Emilio Sulis, Delia Irazú Hernández Farías, Paolo Rosso, Viviana Patti, and Giancarlo Ruffo. 2016. Figurative messages and affect in Twitter: Differences between #irony, #sarcasm and #not. Knowledge-Based Systems. In Press. Available online.Maite Taboada and Jack Grieve. 2004. Analyzing appraisal automatically. In Proceedings of the AAAI Spring Symposium on Exploring Attitude and Affect in Text: Theories and Applications. AAAI, Stanford, CA, 158--161.Yi-jie Tang and Hsin-Hsi Chen. 2014. Chinese irony corpus construction and ironic structure analysis. In Proceedings of the 25th International Conference on Computational Linguistics (COLING’14). Association for Computational Linguistics, Dublin, Ireland, 1269--1278.Tony Veale and Yanfen Hao. 2010. Detecting ironic intent in creative comparisons. In Proceedings of the 19th European Conference on Artificial Intelligence. IOS Press, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 765--770.Byron C. Wallace. 2015. Computational irony: A survey and new perspectives. Artificial Intelligence Review 43, 4 (2015), 467--483.Byron C. Wallace, Do Kook Choe, and Eugene Charniak. 2015. Sparse, contextually informed models for irony detection: Exploiting user communities, entities and sentiment. In Proceedings of the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics and the 7th International Joint Conference on Natural Language Processing (Volume 1: Long Papers). Association for Computational Linguistics, Beijing, China, 1035--1044.Angela P. Wang. 2013. #Irony or #sarcasm—a quantitative and qualitative study based on twitter. In Proceedings of the 27th Pacific Asia Conference on Language, Information, and Computation (PACLIC’13). Department of English, National Chengchi University, Taipei, Taiwan, 349--356.Juanita M. Whalen, Penny M. Pexman, J. Alastair Gill, and Scott Nowson. 2013. Verbal irony use in personal blogs. Behaviour & Information Technology 32, 6 (2013), 560--569.Cynthia Whissell. 2009. Using the revised dictionary of affect in language to quantify the emotional undertones of samples of natural languages. Psychological Reports 2, 105 (2009), 509--521.Deirdre Wilson and Dan Sperber. 1992. On verbal irony. Lingua 87, 1--2 (1992), 53--76.Theresa Wilson, Janyce Wiebe, and Paul Hoffmann. 2005. Recognizing contextual polarity in phrase-level sentiment analysis. In Proceedings of the Conference on Human Language Technology and Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (HLT’05). Association for Computational Linguistics, Stroudsburg, PA, 347--354.Alecia Wolf. 2000. Emotional expression online: Gender differences in emoticon use. CyberPsychology & Behavior 3, 5 (2000), 827--833.Zhibiao Wu and Martha Palmer. 1994. Verbs semantics and lexical selection. In Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics (ACL’94). Association for Computational Linguistics, Stroudsburg, PA, 133--138

    Hermeneutics and Nature

    Get PDF
    This paper contributes to the on-going research into the ways in which the humanities transformed the natural sciences in the late Eighteenth and early Nineteenth Centuries. By investigating the relationship between hermeneutics -- as developed by Herder -- and natural history, it shows how the methods used for the study of literary and artistic works played a crucial role in the emergence of key natural-scientific fields, including geography and ecology

    Effects of a robot-assisted training of grasp and pronation/supination in chronic stroke: a pilot study

    Get PDF
    <p>Abstract</p> <p>Background</p> <p>Rehabilitation of hand function is challenging, and only few studies have investigated robot-assisted rehabilitation focusing on distal joints of the upper limb. This paper investigates the feasibility of using the <it>HapticKnob</it>, a table-top end-effector device, for robot-assisted rehabilitation of grasping and forearm pronation/supination, two important functions for activities of daily living involving the hand, and which are often impaired in chronic stroke patients. It evaluates the effectiveness of this device for improving hand function and the transfer of improvement to arm function.</p> <p>Methods</p> <p>A single group of fifteen chronic stroke patients with impaired arm and hand functions (Fugl-Meyer motor assessment scale (FM) 10-45/66) participated in a 6-week 3-hours/week rehabilitation program with the <it>HapticKnob</it>. Outcome measures consisted primarily of the FM and Motricity Index (MI) and their respective subsections related to distal and proximal arm function, and were assessed at the beginning, end of treatment and in a 6-weeks follow-up.</p> <p>Results</p> <p>Thirteen subjects successfully completed robot-assisted therapy, with significantly improved hand and arm motor functions, demonstrated by an average 3.00 points increase on the FM and 4.55 on the MI at the completion of the therapy (4.85 FM and 6.84 MI six weeks post-therapy). Improvements were observed both in distal and proximal components of the clinical scales at the completion of the study (2.00 FM wrist/hand, 2.55 FM shoulder/elbow, 2.23 MI hand and 4.23 MI shoulder/elbow). In addition, improvements in hand function were observed, as measured by the Motor Assessment Scale, grip force, and a decrease in arm muscle spasticity. These results were confirmed by motion data collected by the robot.</p> <p>Conclusions</p> <p>The results of this study show the feasibility of this robot-assisted therapy with patients presenting a large range of impairment levels. A significant homogeneous improvement in both hand and arm function was observed, which was maintained 6 weeks after end of the therapy.</p
    corecore