40 research outputs found

    THETA-rhythm makes the world go round:dissociative effects of TMS theta versus alpha entrainment of right pTPJ on embodied perspective transformations

    Get PDF
    Being able to imagine another person's experience and perspective of the world is a crucial human ability and recent reports suggest that humans "embody" another's viewpoint by mentally rotating their own body representation into the other's orientation. Our recent Magnetoencephalography (MEG) data further confirmed this notion of embodied perspective transformations and pinpointed the right posterior temporo-parietal junction (pTPJ) as the crucial hub in a distributed network oscillating at theta frequency (3-7 Hz). In a subsequent transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) experiment we interfered with right pTPJ processing and observed a modulation of the embodied aspects of perspective transformations. While these results corroborated the role of right pTPJ, the notion of theta oscillations being the crucial neural code remained a correlational observation based on our MEG data. In the current study we therefore set out to confirm the importance of theta oscillations directly by means of TMS entrainment. We compared entrainment of right pTPJ at 6 Hz vs. 10 Hz and confirmed that only 6 Hz entrainment facilitated embodied perspective transformations (at 160° angular disparity) while 10 Hz slowed it down. The reverse was true at low angular disparity (60° between egocentric and target perspective) where a perspective transformation was not strictly necessary. Our results further corroborate right pTPJ involvement in embodied perspective transformations and highlight theta oscillations as a crucial neural code

    Short-term impacts of Universal Basic Income on population mental health inequalities in the UK: a microsimulation modelling study

    Get PDF
    Background: Population mental health in the United Kingdom (UK) has deteriorated, alongside worsening socioeconomic conditions, over the last decade. Policies such as Universal Basic Income (UBI) have been suggested as an alternative economic approach to improve population mental health and reduce health inequalities. UBI may improve mental health (MH), but to our knowledge, no studies have trialled or modelled UBI in whole populations. We aimed to estimate the short-term effects of introducing UBI on mental health in the UK working-age population. Methods and findings: Adults aged 25 to 64 years were simulated across a 4-year period from 2022 to 2026 with the SimPaths microsimulation model, which models the effects of UK tax/benefit policies on mental health via income, poverty, and employment transitions. Data from the nationally representative UK Household Longitudinal Study were used to generate the simulated population (n = 25,000) and causal effect estimates. Three counterfactual UBI scenarios were modelled from 2023: “Partial” (value equivalent to existing benefits), “Full” (equivalent to the UK Minimum Income Standard), and “Full+” (retaining means-tested benefits for disability, housing, and childcare). Likely common mental disorder (CMD) was measured using the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12, score ≄4). Relative and slope indices of inequality were calculated, and outcomes stratified by gender, age, education, and household structure. Simulations were run 1,000 times to generate 95% uncertainty intervals (UIs). Sensitivity analyses relaxed SimPaths assumptions about reduced employment resulting from Full/Full+ UBI. Partial UBI had little impact on poverty, employment, or mental health. Full UBI scenarios practically eradicated poverty but decreased employment (for Full+ from 78.9% [95% UI 77.9, 79.9] to 74.1% [95% UI 72.6, 75.4]). Full+ UBI increased absolute CMD prevalence by 0.38% (percentage points; 95% UI 0.13, 0.69) in 2023, equivalent to 157,951 additional CMD cases (95% UI 54,036, 286,805); effects were largest for men (0.63% [95% UI 0.31, 1.01]) and those with children (0.64% [95% UI 0.18, 1.14]). In our sensitivity analysis assuming minimal UBI-related employment impacts, CMD prevalence instead fell by 0.27% (95% UI −0.49, −0.05), a reduction of 112,228 cases (95% UI 20,783, 203,673); effects were largest for women (−0.32% [95% UI −0.65, 0.00]), those without children (−0.40% [95% UI −0.68, −0.15]), and those with least education (−0.42% [95% UI −0.97, 0.15]). There was no effect on educational mental health inequalities in any scenario, and effects waned by 2026. The main limitations of our methods are the model’s short time horizon and focus on pathways from UBI to mental health solely via income, poverty, and employment, as well as the inability to integrate macroeconomic consequences of UBI; future iterations of the model will address these limitations. Conclusions: UBI has potential to improve short-term population mental health by reducing poverty, particularly for women, but impacts are highly dependent on whether individuals choose to remain in employment following its introduction. Future research modelling additional causal pathways between UBI and mental health would be beneficial

    Oscillatory networks of high-level mental alignment::A perspective-taking MEG study

    Get PDF
    Mentally imagining another's perspective is a high-level social process, reliant on manipulating internal representations of the self in an embodied manner. Recently Wang et al. (2016) showed that theta-band (3–7 Hz) brain oscillations within the right temporo-parietal junction (rTPJ) and brain regions coding for motor/body schema contribute to the process of perspective-taking. Using a similar paradigm, we set out to unravel the extended functional brain network in detail. Increasing the angle between self and other perspective was accompanied by longer reaction times and increases in theta power within rTPJ, right lateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and right anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Using Granger-causality, we showed that lateral PFC and ACC exert top-down influence over rTPJ, indicative of executive control processes required for managing conflicts between self and other perspectives. Finally, we quantified patterns of whole-brain phase coupling in relation to the rTPJ. Results suggest that rTPJ increases its theta-band phase synchrony with brain regions involved in mentalizing and regions coding for motor/body schema; whilst decreasing synchrony to visual regions. Implications for neurocognitive models are discussed, and it is proposed that rTPJ acts as a ‘hub’ to route bottom-up visual information to internal representations of the self during perspective-taking, co-ordinated by theta-band oscillations

    Does exercise intensity matter for fatigue during (neo‐)adjuvant cancer treatment? The Phys‐Can randomized clinical trial

    Get PDF
    Exercise during cancer treatment improves cancer‐related fatigue (CRF), but the importance of exercise intensity for CRF is unclear. We compared the effects of high‐ vs low‐to‐moderate‐intensity exercise with or without additional behavior change support (BCS) on CRF in patients undergoing (neo‐)adjuvant cancer treatment. This was a multicenter, 2x2 factorial design randomized controlled trial (Clinical Trials NCT02473003) in Sweden. Participants recently diagnosed with breast (n = 457), prostate (n = 97) or colorectal (n = 23) cancer undergoing (neo‐)adjuvant treatment were randomized to high intensity (n = 144), low‐to‐moderate intensity (n = 144), high intensity with BCS (n = 144) or low‐to‐moderate intensity with BCS (n = 145). The 6‐month exercise intervention included supervised resistance training and home‐based endurance training. CRF was assessed by Multidimensional Fatigue Inventory (MFI, five subscales score range 4‐20), and Functional Assessment of Chronic Illness Therapy‐Fatigue scale (FACIT‐F, score range 0‐52). Multiple linear regression for main factorial effects was performed according to intention‐to‐treat, with post‐intervention CRF as primary endpoint. Overall, 577 participants (mean age 58.7 years) were randomized. Participants randomized to high‐ vs low‐to‐moderate‐intensity exercise had lower physical fatigue (MFI Physical Fatigue subscale; mean difference −1.05 [95% CI: −1.85, −0.25]), but the difference was not clinically important (ie <2). We found no differences in other CRF dimensions and no effect of additional BCS. There were few minor adverse events. For CRF, patients undergoing (neo‐)adjuvant treatment for breast, prostate or colorectal cancer can safely exercise at high‐ or low‐to‐moderate intensity, according to their own preferences. Additional BCS does not provide extra benefit for CRF in supervised, well‐controlled exercise interventions

    Consensus Paper: Cerebellum and Social Cognition.

    Get PDF
    The traditional view on the cerebellum is that it controls motor behavior. Although recent work has revealed that the cerebellum supports also nonmotor functions such as cognition and affect, only during the last 5 years it has become evident that the cerebellum also plays an important social role. This role is evident in social cognition based on interpreting goal-directed actions through the movements of individuals (social "mirroring") which is very close to its original role in motor learning, as well as in social understanding of other individuals' mental state, such as their intentions, beliefs, past behaviors, future aspirations, and personality traits (social "mentalizing"). Most of this mentalizing role is supported by the posterior cerebellum (e.g., Crus I and II). The most dominant hypothesis is that the cerebellum assists in learning and understanding social action sequences, and so facilitates social cognition by supporting optimal predictions about imminent or future social interaction and cooperation. This consensus paper brings together experts from different fields to discuss recent efforts in understanding the role of the cerebellum in social cognition, and the understanding of social behaviors and mental states by others, its effect on clinical impairments such as cerebellar ataxia and autism spectrum disorder, and how the cerebellum can become a potential target for noninvasive brain stimulation as a therapeutic intervention. We report on the most recent empirical findings and techniques for understanding and manipulating cerebellar circuits in humans. Cerebellar circuitry appears now as a key structure to elucidate social interactions

    Short-term impacts of Universal Basic Income on population mental health inequalities in the UK: A microsimulation modelling study.

    No full text
    Table A: Key model assumptions of UKMOD and SimPaths. Table B: Effect estimates for use in Step 2 of SimPaths causal mental health module. Table C: All individual benefits retained and/or suspended in each UBI scenario. Table D: Alternative effect estimates for use in Step 2 of SimPaths causal mental health module during sensitivity analyses. Figure A: Internal validation graphs from the SimPaths GUI contrasting predicted outcomes with observed Understanding Society data from 2011–2017 (yo = years old). Figure B: Cumulative mean prevalence of common mental disorder and poverty by number of model iteratio. Figure C: Prevalence of common mental disorder (CMD) in SimPaths versus the Health Survey for England from 2012–2018. Table E: Population-level economic impacts of Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies modelled in UKMOD. Figure D: Gainers and losers by household income decile (before housing costs) ranging from low to high, with Partial UBI compared with baseline tax/benefit policies in 2023 (Scenario 2). Figure E: Gainers and losers by household income decile (before housing costs) ranging from low to high, with Full UBI compared with baseline tax/benefit policies in 2023 (Scenario 3). Table F: Median income, prevalence of poverty, employment rate, and mean hours worked in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 (95% uncertainty intervals). Table G: Estimated prevalence of common mental disorders (CMD) and mental health inequalities in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 (95% uncertainty intervals). Figure G: Estimated relative (left panel) and slope (right panel) indices of inequality by education for common mental disorder (CMD) in modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022–2026. Table H: Estimated prevalence of common mental disorders (%) in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 stratified by gender, education, age, and household structure (95% uncertainty intervals. Table I: Structural Sensitivity Analyses—Median income, prevalence of poverty, employment rate, and mean hours worked in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 (95% uncertainty intervals). Figure I: Structural Sensitivity Analysis 1, relaxing employment assumptions—Estimated prevalence of common mental disorder (CMD) for modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022–2026. Figure J: Structural Sensitivity Analysis 2, using economic inactivity effects—Estimated prevalence of common mental disorder (CMD) for modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022–2026. Table J: Structural Sensitivity Analyses—Estimated prevalence of common mental disorders and mental health inequalities in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 (95% uncertainty intervals). Figure K: Structural Sensitivity Analysis 1, relaxing employment assumptions—Estimated relative (left panel) and slope (right panel) indices of inequality by education for common mental disorder (CMD) in modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022–2026. Figure L: Structural Sensitivity Analysis 2, using economic inactivity effects—Estimated relative (left panel) and slope (right panel) indices of inequality by education for common mental disorder (CMD) in modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022–2026. Figure M: Structural Sensitivity Analysis 1, relaxing employment assumptions—Estimated prevalence of common mental disorder (CMD) for modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022 to 2026 with 95% uncertainty intervals, stratified by gender (A), education (B), age (C), and household structure (D). Note different scales used for each stratification. Figure N: Structural Sensitivity Analysis 2, using economic inactivity effects—Estimated prevalence of common mental disorder (CMD) for modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022 to 2026 stratified by gender (A), education (B), age (C), and household structure (D). Note different scales used for each stratification. Table K: Structural Sensitivity Analyses—Estimated prevalence of common mental disorders in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 stratified by gender, education, age, previous poverty/employment status, and household structure (95% uncertainty intervals). Table L: Analytical Sensitivity Analyses—Median income, prevalence of poverty, and prevalence of unemployment in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 (95% uncertainty intervals). Figure O: Analytical Sensitivity Analysis, using alternative estimates from systematic reviews—Estimated prevalence of common mental disorder (CMD) for modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022–2026. Table M: Analytical Sensitivity Analyses—Prevalence of common mental disorders and mental health inequalities in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 (95% uncertainty intervals). Figure P: Analytical Sensitivity Analysis, using alternative estimates from systematic reviews—Estimated relative (left panel) and slope (right panel) indices of inequality by education for common mental disorder (CMD) in modelled Universal Basic Income (UBI) policies from 2022–2026. Table N: Estimated GHQ Likert score in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 (95% uncertainty intervals). Table O: Estimated GHQ Likert score in baseline scenario and three simulated Universal Basic Income (UBI) scenarios from 2022–2026 stratified by gender, education, age, previous poverty/employment status, and household structure (95% uncertainty intervals). (PDF)</p
    corecore