165 research outputs found

    Practice effects in healthy adults: A longitudinal study on frequent repetitive cognitive testing

    Get PDF
    <p>Abstract</p> <p>Background</p> <p>Cognitive deterioration is a core symptom of many neuropsychiatric disorders and target of increasing significance for novel treatment strategies. Hence, its reliable capture in long-term follow-up studies is prerequisite for recording the natural course of diseases and for estimating potential benefits of therapeutic interventions. Since repeated neuropsychological testing is required for respective longitudinal study designs, occurrence, time pattern and magnitude of practice effects on cognition have to be understood first under healthy good-performance conditions to enable design optimization and result interpretation in disease trials.</p> <p>Methods</p> <p>Healthy adults (N = 36; 47.3 ± 12.0 years; mean IQ 127.0 ± 14.1; 58% males) completed 7 testing sessions, distributed asymmetrically from high to low frequency, over 1 year (baseline, weeks 2-3, 6, 9, months 3, 6, 12). The neuropsychological test battery covered 6 major cognitive domains by several well-established tests each.</p> <p>Results</p> <p>Most tests exhibited a similar pattern upon repetition: (1) Clinically relevant practice effects during high-frequency testing until month 3 (Cohen's <it>d </it>0.36-1.19), most pronounced early on, and (2) a performance plateau thereafter upon low-frequency testing. Few tests were non-susceptible to practice or limited by ceiling effects. Influence of confounding variables (age, IQ, personality) was minor.</p> <p>Conclusions</p> <p>Practice effects are prominent particularly in the early phase of high-frequency repetitive cognitive testing of healthy well-performing subjects. An optimal combination and timing of tests, as extractable from this study, will aid in controlling their impact. Moreover, normative data for serial testing may now be collected to assess normal learning curves as important comparative readout of pathological cognitive processes.</p

    Categorical Perception of Fear and Anger Expressions in Whole, Masked and Composite Faces

    Get PDF
    Wegrzyn M, Bruckhaus I, Kißler J. Categorical Perception of Fear and Anger Expressions in Whole, Masked and Composite Faces. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(8): e0134790.Human observers are remarkably proficient at recognizing expressions of emotions and at readily grouping them into distinct categories. When morphing one facial expression into another, the linear changes in low-level features are insufficient to describe the changes in perception, which instead follow an s-shaped function. Important questions are, whether there are single diagnostic regions in the face that drive categorical perception for certain parings of emotion expressions, and how information in those regions interacts when presented together. We report results from two experiments with morphed fear-anger expressions, where (a) half of the face was masked or (b) composite faces made up of different expressions were presented. When isolated upper and lower halves of faces were shown, the eyes were found to be almost as diagnostic as the whole face, with the response function showing a steep category boundary. In contrast, the mouth allowed for a substantially lesser amount of accuracy and responses followed a much flatter psychometric function. When a composite face consisting of mismatched upper and lower halves was used and observers were instructed to exclusively judge either the expression of mouth or eyes, the to-be-ignored part always influenced perception of the target region. In line with experiment 1, the eye region exerted a much stronger influence on mouth judgements than vice versa. Again, categorical perception was significantly more pronounced for upper halves of faces. The present study shows that identification of fear and anger in morphed faces relies heavily on information from the upper half of the face, most likely the eye region. Categorical perception is possible when only the upper face half is present, but compromised when only the lower part is shown. Moreover, observers tend to integrate all available features of a face, even when trying to focus on only one part

    It's all in your head - how anticipating evaluation affects the processing of emotional trait adjectives

    Get PDF
    Schindler S, Wegrzyn M, Steppacher I, Kißler J. It's all in your head - how anticipating evaluation affects the processing of emotional trait adjectives. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:1292.Language has an intrinsically evaluative and communicative function. Words can serve to describe emotional traits and states in others and communicate evaluations. Using electroencephalography (EEG), we investigate how the cerebral processing of emotional trait adjectives is modulated by their perceived communicative sender in anticipation of an evaluation. 16 students were videotaped while they described themselves. They were told that a stranger would evaluate their personality based on this recording by endorsing trait adjectives. In a control condition a computer program supposedly randomly selected the adjectives. Actually, both conditions were random. A larger parietal N1 was found for adjectives in the supposedly human-generated condition. This indicates that more visual attention is allocated to the presented adjectives when putatively interacting with a human. Between 400 and 700 ms a fronto-central main effect of emotion was found. Positive, and in tendency also negative adjectives, led to a larger late positive potential (LPP) compared to neutral adjectives. A centro-parietal interaction in the LPP-window was due to larger LPP amplitudes for negative compared to neutral adjectives within the ‘human sender’ condition. Larger LPP amplitudes are related to stimulus elaboration and memory consolidation. Participants responded more to emotional content particularly when presented in a meaningful ‘human’ context. This was first observed in the early posterior negativity window (210–260 ms). But the significant interaction between sender and emotion reached only trend-level on post hoc tests. Our results specify differential effects of even implied communicative partners on emotional language processing. They show that anticipating evaluation by a communicative partner alone is sufficient to increase the relevance of particularly emotional adjectives, given a seemingly realistic interactive setting

    Mapping the emotional face. How individual face parts contribute to successful emotion recognition

    Get PDF
    Wegrzyn M, Vogt M, Kireclioglu B, Schneider J, Kißler J. Mapping the emotional face. How individual face parts contribute to successful emotion recognition. PLOS ONE. 2017;12(5): e0177239.Which facial features allow human observers to successfully recognize expressions of emotion? While the eyes and mouth have been frequently shown to be of high importance, research on facial action units has made more precise predictions about the areas involved in displaying each emotion. The present research investigated on a fine-grained level, which physical features are most relied on when decoding facial expressions. In the experiment, individual faces expressing the basic emotions according to Ekman were hidden behind a mask of 48 tiles, which was sequentially uncovered. Participants were instructed to stop the sequence as soon as they recognized the facial expression and assign it the correct label. For each part of the face, its contribution to successful recognition was computed, allowing to visualize the importance of different face areas for each expression. Overall, observers were mostly relying on the eye and mouth regions when successfully recognizing an emotion. Furthermore, the difference in the importance of eyes and mouth allowed to group the expressions in a continuous space, ranging from sadness and fear (reliance on the eyes) to disgust and happiness (mouth). The face parts with highest diagnostic value for expression identification were typically located in areas corresponding to action units from the facial action coding system. A similarity analysis of the usefulness of different face parts for expression recognition demonstrated that faces cluster according to the emotion they express, rather than by low-level physical features. Also, expressions relying more on the eyes or mouth region were in close proximity in the constructed similarity space. These analyses help to better understand how human observers process expressions of emotion, by delineating the mapping from facial features to psychological representation

    Quantifying the Confidence in fMRI-Based Language Lateralisation Through Laterality Index Deconstruction

    Get PDF
    Wegrzyn M, Mertens M, Bien C, Woermann FG, Labudda K. Quantifying the Confidence in fMRI-Based Language Lateralisation Through Laterality Index Deconstruction. Frontiers in Neurology. 2019;10: 655.In epilepsy patients, language lateralisation is an important part of the presurgical diagnostic process. Using task-based fMRI, language lateralisation can be determined by visual inspection of activity patterns or by quantifying the difference in left- and right-hemisphere activity using variations of a basic formula [(L-R)/(L+R)]. However, the values of this laterality index (LI) depend on the choice of activity thresholds and regions of interest. The diagnostic utility of the U also depends on how its continuous values are translated into categorical decisions about a patient's language lateralisation. Here, we analysed fMRI data from 712 epilepsy patients who performed a verbal fluency task. Each fMRI data set was evaluated by a trained human rater as depicting left-sided, right-sided, or bilateral lateralisation or as being inconclusive. We used data-driven methods to define the activity thresholds and regions of interest used for LI computation and to define a classification scheme that allowed us to translate the U values into categorical decisions. By deconstructing the LI into measures of laterality (L-R) and strength (L+R), we also modelled the relationship between activation strength and conclusiveness of a data set. In a held-out data set, predictions reached 91% correct when using only conclusive data and 82% when inconclusive data were included. Although only trained on human evaluations of fMRIs, the approach generalised to the prediction of language Wada test results, allowing for significant above-chance accuracies. Compared against different existing methods of U-computation, our approach improved the identification and exclusion of inconclusive cases and ensured that decisions for the remaining data could be made with consistently high accuracies. We discuss how this approach can support clinicians in assessing fMRI data on a single-case level, deciding whether lateralisation can be determined with sufficient certainty or whether additional information is needed

    The hidden identity of faces. A case of lifelong prosopagnosia

    Get PDF
    Wegrzyn M, Garlichs A, Heß RWK, Woermann FG, Labudda K. The hidden identity of faces. A case of lifelong prosopagnosia. BMC Psychology. 2019;7(1): 4.Background Not being able to recognize a person’s face is a highly debilitating condition from which people with developmental prosopagnosia (DP) suffer their entire life. Here we describe the case of J, a 30 year old woman who reports being unable to recognize her parents, her husband, or herself in the mirror. Case presentation We set out to assess the severity of J’s prosopagnosia using tests with unfamiliar as well as familiar faces and investigated whether impaired configural processing explains her deficit. To assess the specificity of the impairment, we tested J’s performance when evaluating emotions, intentions, and the attractiveness and likability of faces. Detailed testing revealed typical brain activity patterns for faces and normal object recognition skills, and no evidence of any brain injury. However, compared to a group of matched controls, J showed severe deficits in learning new faces, and in recognizing familiar faces when only inner features were available. Her recognition of uncropped faces with blurred features was within the normal range, indicating preserved configural processing when peripheral features are available. J was also unimpaired when evaluating intentions and emotions in faces. In line with healthy controls, J rated more average faces as more attractive. However, she was the only one to rate them as less likable, indicating a preference for more distinctive and easier to recognize faces. Conclusions Taken together, the results illustrate both the severity and the specificity of DP in a single case. While DP is a heterogeneous disorder, an inability to integrate the inner features of the face into a whole might be the best explanation for the difficulties many individuals with prosopagnosia experience

    Thermal and electrical conductivity of melt mixed polycarbonate hybrid composites co-filled with multi-walled carbon nanotubes and graphene nanoplatelets

    Full text link
    "This is the peer reviewed version of the following article: Wegrzyn, M., Ortega, A., Benedito, A., & Gimenez, E. (2015). Thermal and electrical conductivity of melt mixed polycarbonate hybrid composites co&#8208;filled with multi&#8208;walled carbon nanotubes and graphene nanoplatelets. Journal of Applied Polymer Science, 132(37), which has been published in final form at https://doi.org/10.1002/app.42536. This article may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance with Wiley Terms and Conditions for Self-Archiving."[EN] In this work, we present thermoplastic nanocomposites of polycarbonate (PC) matrix with hybrid nanofillers system formed by a melt-mixing approach. Various concentrations of multi-walled carbon nanotubes (MWCNT) and graphene nanoplatelets (GnP) were mixed in to PC and the melt was homogenized. The nanocomposites were compression molded and characterized by different techniques. Torque dependence on the nanofiller composition increased with the presence of carbon nanotubes. The synergy of carbon nanotubes and GnP showed exponential increase of thermal conductivity, which was compared to logarithmic increase for nanocomposite with no MWCNT. Decrease of Shore A hardness at elevated loads present for all investigated nanocomposites was correlated with the expected low homogeneity caused by a low shear during melt-mixing. Mathematical model was used to calculate elastic modulus from Shore A tests results. Vicat softening temperature (VST) showed opposite pattern for hybrid nanocomposites and for PC-MWCNT increasing in the latter case. Electrical conductivity boost was explained by the collective effect of high nanofiller loads and synergy of MWCNT and GnP. (c) 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. J. Appl. Polym. Sci. 2015, 132, 42536.This work is funded by the European Community's Seventh Framework Program (FP7-PEOPLE-ITN-2008) within the CONTACT project Marie Curie Fellowship under grant number 238363.Wegrzyn, M.; Ortega, A.; Benedito, A.; Giménez Torres, E. (2015). Thermal and electrical conductivity of melt mixed polycarbonate hybrid composites co-filled with multi-walled carbon nanotubes and graphene nanoplatelets. Journal of Applied Polymer Science. 132(37):42536-1-42536-8. https://doi.org/10.1002/app.42536S42536-142536-813237Su, D. S., & Schlögl, R. (2010). Nanostructured Carbon and Carbon Nanocomposites for Electrochemical Energy Storage Applications. ChemSusChem, 3(2), 136-168. doi:10.1002/cssc.200900182Yang, L., Liu, F., Xia, H., Qian, X., Shen, K., & Zhang, J. (2011). Improving the electrical conductivity of a carbon nanotube/polypropylene composite by vibration during injection-moulding. Carbon, 49(10), 3274-3283. doi:10.1016/j.carbon.2011.03.054Singh, I. V., Tanaka, M., & Endo, M. (2007). Effect of interface on the thermal conductivity of carbon nanotube composites. International Journal of Thermal Sciences, 46(9), 842-847. doi:10.1016/j.ijthermalsci.2006.11.003Kuan, H.-C., Ma, C.-C. M., Chang, W.-P., Yuen, S.-M., Wu, H.-H., & Lee, T.-M. (2005). Synthesis, thermal, mechanical and rheological properties of multiwall carbon nanotube/waterborne polyurethane nanocomposite. Composites Science and Technology, 65(11-12), 1703-1710. doi:10.1016/j.compscitech.2005.02.017Arasteh, R., Omidi, M., Rousta, A. H. A., & Kazerooni, H. (2011). A Study on Effect of Waviness on Mechanical Properties of Multi-Walled Carbon Nanotube/Epoxy Composites Using Modified Halpin–Tsai Theory. Journal of Macromolecular Science, Part B, 50(12), 2464-2480. doi:10.1080/00222348.2011.579868Cai, D., Jin, J., Yusoh, K., Rafiq, R., & Song, M. (2012). High performance polyurethane/functionalized graphene nanocomposites with improved mechanical and thermal properties. Composites Science and Technology, 72(6), 702-707. doi:10.1016/j.compscitech.2012.01.020Yu, D., & Dai, L. (2009). Self-Assembled Graphene/Carbon Nanotube Hybrid Films for Supercapacitors. The Journal of Physical Chemistry Letters, 1(2), 467-470. doi:10.1021/jz9003137Haslam, M. D., & Raeymaekers, B. (2013). A composite index to quantify dispersion of carbon nanotubes in polymer-based composite materials. Composites Part B: Engineering, 55, 16-21. doi:10.1016/j.compositesb.2013.05.038Pötschke, P., Dudkin, S. M., & Alig, I. (2003). Dielectric spectroscopy on melt processed polycarbonate—multiwalled carbon nanotube composites. Polymer, 44(17), 5023-5030. doi:10.1016/s0032-3861(03)00451-8Stankovich, S., Dikin, D. A., Dommett, G. H. B., Kohlhaas, K. M., Zimney, E. J., Stach, E. A., … Ruoff, R. S. (2006). Graphene-based composite materials. Nature, 442(7100), 282-286. doi:10.1038/nature04969Sathyanarayana, S., Olowojoba, G., Weiss, P., Caglar, B., Pataki, B., Mikonsaari, I., … Henning, F. (2012). Compounding of MWCNTs with PS in a Twin-Screw Extruder with Varying Process Parameters: Morphology, Interfacial Behavior, Thermal Stability, Rheology, and Volume Resistivity. Macromolecular Materials and Engineering, 298(1), 89-105. doi:10.1002/mame.201200018Ye, L., Wu, Q., & Qu, B. (2009). Synergistic effects and mechanism of multiwalled carbon nanotubes with magnesium hydroxide in halogen-free flame retardant EVA/MH/MWNT nanocomposites. Polymer Degradation and Stability, 94(5), 751-756. doi:10.1016/j.polymdegradstab.2009.02.010Kalaitzidou, K., Fukushima, H., & Drzal, L. T. (2007). Multifunctional polypropylene composites produced by incorporation of exfoliated graphite nanoplatelets. Carbon, 45(7), 1446-1452. doi:10.1016/j.carbon.2007.03.029Mu, Q., Feng, S., & Diao, G. (2007). Thermal conductivity of silicone rubber filled with ZnO. Polymer Composites, 28(2), 125-130. doi:10.1002/pc.20276Pötschke, P., Bhattacharyya, A. R., & Janke, A. (2004). Melt mixing of polycarbonate with multiwalled carbon nanotubes: microscopic studies on the state of dispersion. European Polymer Journal, 40(1), 137-148. doi:10.1016/j.eurpolymj.2003.08.008King, J. A., Barton, R. L., Hauser, R. A., & Keith, J. M. (2008). Synergistic effects of carbon fillers in electrically and thermally conductive liquid crystal polymer based resins. Polymer Composites, 29(4), 421-428. doi:10.1002/pc.20446Hwang, Y., Kim, M., & Kim, J. (2013). Improvement of the mechanical properties and thermal conductivity of poly(ether-ether-ketone) with the addition of graphene oxide-carbon nanotube hybrid fillers. Composites Part A: Applied Science and Manufacturing, 55, 195-202. doi:10.1016/j.compositesa.2013.08.010Babaei, H., Keblinski, P., & Khodadadi, J. M. (2013). Thermal conductivity enhancement of paraffins by increasing the alignment of molecules through adding CNT/graphene. International Journal of Heat and Mass Transfer, 58(1-2), 209-216. doi:10.1016/j.ijheatmasstransfer.2012.11.013Yang, S.-Y., Lin, W.-N., Huang, Y.-L., Tien, H.-W., Wang, J.-Y., Ma, C.-C. M., … Wang, Y.-S. (2011). Synergetic effects of graphene platelets and carbon nanotubes on the mechanical and thermal properties of epoxy composites. Carbon, 49(3), 793-803. doi:10.1016/j.carbon.2010.10.014Pascual, J., Peris, F., Boronat, T., Fenollar, O., & Balart, R. (2011). Study of the effects of multi-walled carbon nanotubes on mechanical performance and thermal stability of polypropylene. Polymer Engineering & Science, 52(4), 733-740. doi:10.1002/pen.22128Yasin, T., Nisar, M., Shafiq, M., Nho, Y.-C., & Ahmad, R. (2013). Influence of sepiolite and electron beam irradiation on the structural and physicochemical properties of polyethylene/starch nanocomposites. Polymer Composites, 34(3), 408-416. doi:10.1002/pc.22431Zhang, W. D., Shen, L., Phang, I. Y., & Liu, T. (2004). Carbon Nanotubes Reinforced Nylon-6 Composite Prepared by Simple Melt-Compounding. Macromolecules, 37(2), 256-259. doi:10.1021/ma035594fZhang, C., Tjiu, W. W., Liu, T., Lui, W. Y., Phang, I. Y., & Zhang, W.-D. (2011). Dramatically Enhanced Mechanical Performance of Nylon-6 Magnetic Composites with Nanostructured Hybrid One-Dimensional Carbon Nanotube−Two-Dimensional Clay Nanoplatelet Heterostructures. The Journal of Physical Chemistry B, 115(13), 3392-3399. doi:10.1021/jp112284kLin, J., Wang, L., & Chen, G. (2010). Modification of Graphene Platelets and their Tribological Properties as a Lubricant Additive. Tribology Letters, 41(1), 209-215. doi:10.1007/s11249-010-9702-5Qiu, L., Yang, X., Gou, X., Yang, W., Ma, Z.-F., Wallace, G. G., & Li, D. (2010). Dispersing Carbon Nanotubes with Graphene Oxide in Water and Synergistic Effects between Graphene Derivatives. Chemistry - A European Journal, 16(35), 10653-10658. doi:10.1002/chem.201001771Tian, L., Meziani, M. J., Lu, F., Kong, C. Y., Cao, L., Thorne, T. J., & Sun, Y.-P. (2010). Graphene Oxides for Homogeneous Dispersion of Carbon Nanotubes. ACS Applied Materials & Interfaces, 2(11), 3217-3222. doi:10.1021/am100687nPotts, J. R., Dreyer, D. R., Bielawski, C. W., & Ruoff, R. S. (2011). Graphene-based polymer nanocomposites. Polymer, 52(1), 5-25. doi:10.1016/j.polymer.2010.11.042Song, Y. S., & Youn, J. R. (2005). Influence of dispersion states of carbon nanotubes on physical properties of epoxy nanocomposites. Carbon, 43(7), 1378-1385. doi:10.1016/j.carbon.2005.01.007Huang, H., Liu, C. H., Wu, Y., & Fan, S. (2005). Aligned Carbon Nanotube Composite Films for Thermal Management. Advanced Materials, 17(13), 1652-1656. doi:10.1002/adma.200500467Shokrieh, M. M., Hosseinkhani, M. R., Naimi-Jamal, M. R., & Tourani, H. (2013). Nanoindentation and nanoscratch investigations on graphene-based nanocomposites. Polymer Testing, 32(1), 45-51. doi:10.1016/j.polymertesting.2012.09.001Schiøtz , J. nd Dinesen , A. R. 2001 127Hornbostel, B., Pötschke, P., Kotz, J., & Roth, S. (2008). Mechanical properties of triple composites of polycarbonate, single-walled carbon nanotubes and carbon fibres. Physica E: Low-dimensional Systems and Nanostructures, 40(7), 2434-2439. doi:10.1016/j.physe.2007.08.100Gupta, T. K., Singh, B. P., Mathur, R. B., & Dhakate, S. R. (2014). Multi-walled carbon nanotube–graphene–polyaniline multiphase nanocomposite with superior electromagnetic shielding effectiveness. Nanoscale, 6(2), 842-851. doi:10.1039/c3nr04565jPan, M., Shi, X., Li, X., Hu, H., & Zhang, L. (2004). Morphology and properties of PVC/clay nanocomposites viain situ emulsion polymerization. Journal of Applied Polymer Science, 94(1), 277-286. doi:10.1002/app.20896Wegrzyn, M., Juan, S., Benedito, A., & Giménez, E. (2013). The influence of injection molding parameters on electrical properties of PC/ABS-MWCNT nanocomposites. Journal of Applied Polymer Science, 130(3), 2152-2158. doi:10.1002/app.39412Godavarti, S., & Karwe, M. (1997). Determination of Specific Mechanical Energy Distribution on a Twin-Screw Extruder. Journal of Agricultural Engineering Research, 67(4), 277-287. doi:10.1006/jaer.1997.017

    Mitochondria-Targeted Honokiol Confers a Striking Inhibitory Effect on Lung Cancer via Inhibiting Complex I Activity

    Get PDF
    WOS:000449721600016We synthesized a mitochondria-targeted honokiol (Mito-HNK) that facilitates its mitochondrial accumulation; this dramatically increases its potency and efficacy against highly metastatic lung cancer lines in vitro, and in orthotopic lung tumor xenografts and brain metastases in vivo. Mito-HNK is \textgreater100-fold more potent than HNK in inhibiting cell proliferation, inhibiting mitochondrial complex \textbar, stimulating reactive oxygen species generation, oxidizing mitochondrial peroxiredoxin-3, and suppressing the phosphorylation of mitoSTAT3. Within lung cancer brain metastases in mice, MitoHNK induced the mediators of cell death and decreased the pathways that support invasion and proliferation. In contrast, in the non-malignant stroma, Mito-HNK suppressed pathways that support metastatic lesions, including those involved in inflammation and angiogenesis. Mito-HNK showed no toxicity and targets the metabolic vulnerabilities of primary and metastatic lung cancers. Its pronounced anti-invasive and anti-metastatic effects in the brain are particularly intriguing given the paucity of treatment options for such patients either alone or in combination with standard chemotherapeutics

    Thought experiment: Decoding cognitive processes from the fMRI data of one individual

    Get PDF
    Wegrzyn M, Aust J, Barnstorf L, et al. Thought experiment: Decoding cognitive processes from the fMRI data of one individual. PLOS ONE. 2018;13(9): e0204338.Cognitive processes, such as the generation of language, can be mapped onto the brain using fMRI. These maps can in turn be used for decoding the respective processes from the brain activation patterns. Given individual variations in brain anatomy and organization, analyzes on the level of the single person are important to improve our understanding of how cognitive processes correspond to patterns of brain activity. They also allow to advance clinical applications of fMRI, because in the clinical setting making diagnoses for single cases is imperative. In the present study, we used mental imagery tasks to investigate language production, motor functions, visuo-spatial memory, face processing, and resting-state activity in a single person. Analysis methods were based on similarity metrics, including correlations between training and test data, as well as correlations with maps from the NeuroSynth meta-analysis. The goal was to make accurate predictions regarding the cognitive domain (e.g. language) and the specific content (e.g. animal names) of single 30-second blocks. Four teams used the dataset, each blinded regarding the true labels of the test data. Results showed that the similarity metrics allowed to reach the highest degrees of accuracy when predicting the cognitive domain of a block. Overall, 23 of the 25 test blocks could be correctly predicted by three of the four teams. Excluding the unspecific rest condition, up to 10 out of 20 blocks could be successfully decoded regarding their specific content. The study shows how the information contained in a single fMRI session and in each of its single blocks can allow to draw inferences about the cognitive processes an individual engaged in. Simple methods like correlations between blocks of fMRI data can serve as highly reliable approaches for cognitive decoding. We discuss the implications of our results in the context of clinical fMRI applications, with a focus on how decoding can support functional localization
    • …
    corecore