15 research outputs found

    2-(2-Methyl­naphtho[2,1-b]furan-1-yl)acetic acid

    Get PDF
    In the title mol­ecule, C15H12O3, the two six-membered and one five-membered fused-ring system is almost planar and the CH2C(=O)OH residue is essentially orthogonal to it. In the crystal structure, centrosymmetric dimers are formed via the carboxylic acid {⋯O=C—O—H}2 synthon

    Associations and propositions: the case for a dual-process account of learning in humans

    Get PDF
    Copyright © 2013 Elsevier. NOTICE: This is the author’s version of a work accepted for publication by Elsevier. Changes resulting from the publishing process, including peer review, editing, corrections, structural formatting and other quality control mechanisms, may not be reflected in this document. Changes may have been made to this work since it was submitted for publication. A definitive version was subsequently published in Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 2014, vol. 108, pp. 185 – 195 DOI: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.09.014We review evidence that supports the conclusion that people can and do learn in two distinct ways - one associative, the other propositional. No one disputes that we solve problems by testing hypotheses and inducing underlying rules, so the issue amounts to deciding whether there is evidence that we (and other animals) also rely on a simpler, associative system, that detects the frequency of occurrence of different events in our environment and the contingencies between them. There is neuroscientific evidence that associative learning occurs in at least some animals (e.g., Aplysia californica), so it must be the case that associative learning has evolved. Since both associative and propositional theories can in principle account for many instances of successful learning, the problem is then to show that there are at least some cases where the two classes of theory predict different outcomes. We offer a demonstration of cue competition effects in humans under incidental conditions as evidence against the argument that all such effects are based on cognitive inference. The latter supposition would imply that if the necessary information is unavailable to inference then no cue competition should occur. We then discuss the case of unblocking by reinforcer omission, where associative theory predicts an irrational solution to the problem, and consider the phenomenon of the Perruchet effect, in which conscious expectancy and conditioned response dissociate. Further discussion makes use of evidence that people will sometimes provide one solution to a problem when it is presented to them in summary form, and another when they are presented in rapid succession with trial-by trial information. We also demonstrate that people trained on a discrimination may show a peak shift (predicted by associative theory), but given the time and opportunity to detect the relationships between S+ and S-, show rule-based behavior instead. Finally, we conclude by presenting evidence that research on individual differences suggests that variation in intelligence and explicit problem solving ability are quite unrelated to variation in implicit (associative) learning, and briefly consider the computational implications of our argument, by asking how both associative and propositional processes can be accommodated within a single framework for cognition.ESR

    White Matter Microstructure Predicts Autistic Traits in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

    Get PDF
    Traits of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have previously been found to index clinical severity. This study examined the association of ASD traits with diffusion parameters in adolescent males with ADHD (n = 17), and also compared WM microstructure relative to controls (n = 17). Significant associations (p < 0.05, corrected) were found between fractional anisotropy/radial diffusivity and ASD trait severity (positive and negative correlations respectively), mostly in the right posterior limb of the internal capsule/corticospinal tract, right cerebellar peduncle and the midbrain. No case–control differences were found for the diffusion parameters investigated. This is the first report of a WM microstructural signature of autistic traits in ADHD. Thus, even in the absence of full disorder, ASD traits may index a distinctive underlying neurobiology in ADHD

    Reduced anisotropy in the middle cerebellar peduncle in Chiari-II malformation

    No full text
    Besides supratentorial abnormalities, spina bifida menigomyelocele (SBM) is typically associated with Chiari-II malformation comprising a small cerebellum, which herniates downward due to a shallow posterior fossa. We used diffusion tensor imaging to probe additional microstructural alterations of the major cerebellar white matter tracts, the cerebellar peduncles. A region-of-interest approach was employed in six SBM patients and six matched controls to compare the fractional anisotropy (FA) within the superior, middle, and inferior cerebellar peduncle (SCP, MCP, and ICP, respectively). The FA in the MCP was significantly reduced in the SBM patients (0.44 vs. 0.65, p?=?0.002), while there was no significant difference in the other cerebellar peduncles. In the context of numerous supratentorial white matter abnormalities in SBM such as callosal dysplasia, the most likely explanation of reduced FA in the MCP is a reduced fiber density
    corecore