963 research outputs found

    Role of the medial part of the intraparietal sulcus in implementing movement direction

    Get PDF
    The contribution of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) to visually guided movements has been originally inferred from observations made in patients suffering from optic ataxia. Subsequent electrophysiological studies in monkeys and functional imaging data in humans have corroborated the key role played by the PPC in sensorimotor transformations underlying goal-directed movements, although the exact contribution of this structure remains debated. Here, we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to interfere transiently with the function of the left or right medial part of the intraparietal sulcus (mIPS) in healthy volunteers performing visually guided movements with the right hand. We found that a "virtual lesion" of either mIPS increased the scattering in initial movement direction (DIR), leading to longer trajectory and prolonged movement time, but only when TMS was delivered 100-160 ms before movement onset and for movements directed toward contralateral targets. Control experiments showed that deficits in DIR consequent to mIPS virtual lesions resulted from an inappropriate implementation of the motor command underlying the forthcoming movement and not from an inaccurate computation of the target localization. The present study indicates that mIPS plays a causal role in implementing specifically the direction vector of visually guided movements toward objects situated in the contralateral hemifield

    Willingness towards cognitive engagement: a preliminary study based on a behavioural entropy approach

    Get PDF
    Faced with a novel task some people enthusiastically embark in it and work with determination, while others soon lose interest and progressively reduce their efforts. Although cognitive neuroscience has explored the behavioural and neural features of apathy, the why’s and how’s of positive engagement are only starting to be understood. Stemming from the observation that the left hemisphere is commonly associated to a proactive (‘do something’) disposition, we run a preliminary study exploring the possibility that individual variability in eagerness to engage in cognitive tasks could reflect a preferred left- or right-hemisphere functioning mode. We adapted a task based on response-independent reinforcement and used entropy to characterize the degree of involvement, diversification, and predictability of responses. Entropy was higher in women, who were overall more active, less dependent on instructions, and never reduced their engagement during the task. Conversely, men showed lower entropy, took longer pauses, and became significantly less active by the end of the allotted time, renewing their efforts mainly in response to negative incentives. These findings are discussed in the light of neurobiological data on gender differences in behaviour

    Willingness towards cognitive engagement: a preliminary study based on a behavioural entropy approach

    Get PDF
    Faced with a novel task some people enthusiastically embark in it and work with determination, while others soon lose interest and progressively reduce their efforts. Although cognitive neuroscience has explored the behavioural and neural features of apathy, the why’s and how’s of positive engagement are only starting to be understood. Stemming from the observation that the left hemisphere is commonly associated to a proactive (‘do something’) disposition, we run a preliminary study exploring the possibility that individual variability in eagerness to engage in cognitive tasks could reflect a preferred left- or right-hemisphere functioning mode. We adapted a task based on response-independent reinforcement and used entropy to characterize the degree of involvement, diversification, and predictability of responses. Entropy was higher in women, who were overall more active, less dependent on instructions, and never reduced their engagement during the task. Conversely, men showed lower entropy, took longer pauses, and became significantly less active by the end of the allotted time, renewing their efforts mainly in response to negative incentives. These findings are discussed in the light of neurobiological data on gender differences in behaviour

    Gliomes de bas grade et plasticité cérébrale : Implications fondamentales et cliniques

    Get PDF
    La plasticitĂ© cĂ©rĂ©brale post-lĂ©sionnelle (PCPL) dĂ©crit l’ensemble des processus permettant au systĂšme nerveux central de se rĂ©organiser aprĂšs une atteinte physique. Depuis l’influent travail de Broca et la prise de pouvoir des modĂšles « localisationnistes », il est largement admis que la PCPL est limitĂ©e, voire impossible, au sein des aires fonctionnelles majeures, dites Ă©loquentes. Pourtant, depuis quelques annĂ©es, de nouvelles donnĂ©es issues de la chirurgie des gliomes infiltrants de bas-grade (GIBG) sont venues bousculer ce dogme. Il apparaĂźt en effet de plus en plus clairement que des excisions cĂ©rĂ©brales massives peuvent ĂȘtre intĂ©gralement compensĂ©es, pour ne laisser place Ă  aucun dĂ©ficit fonctionnel dĂ©tectable. Des techniques d’imagerie prĂ©- et post-chirurgicales, ainsi que des procĂ©dures de stimulation peropĂ©ratoire, permettent de suivre la nature et la cinĂ©tique de ces compensations. Celles-ci dĂ©butent avant la chirurgie, en rĂ©action Ă  l’invasion tumorale, et se consolident pendant et aprĂšs la procĂ©dure opĂ©ratoire. Les mĂ©canismes de la compensation prĂ©- et post-lĂ©sionnelle impliquent les aires pĂ©rilĂ©sionnelles, les structures cĂ©rĂ©brales ipsilatĂ©rales distantes et les homologues controlatĂ©raux des zones rĂ©sĂ©quĂ©es. De tels rĂ©sultats ont d’évidentes implications fondamentales et cliniques, et ouvrent d’importantes perspectives pour la comprĂ©hension de la dynamique cĂ©rĂ©brale et des phĂ©nomĂšnes de plasticitĂ©.Post-lesional plasticity (PLP) describes the processes that reorganize cerebral connections after an injury. Since Broca’s influential contribution and the common endorsement of “localisationist” models of brain physiology, it has been widely admitted that PLP was limited, not to say impossible in the so-called “eloquent areas”. However, recent observations associated with the surgical treatments of low grade gliomas have called this dogma into question. Indeed, more and more evidence suggest that large cerebral resections can be compensated so efficiently that no functional deficits can be detected after the surgery. Pre and post surgical investigations based on imaging techniques, as well as intra-surgical investigations involving electrical stimulations, allow to track the nature and the temporal characteristics of these compensations. Compensatory reactions begin before the operation, in response to the tumoral growth. They remain active during and after the surgery. These compensations can involve the perilesional adjacent areas, the distant ipsilateral cerebral structures and the homologous contra-lateral regions. When considered together these results have obvious fundamental and clinical implications. They open new perspectives for understanding cerebral dynamics and the process of brain plasticity

    Forward Modeling Mediates Motor Awareness

    Get PDF
    This chapter focuses on the issue of motor awareness. It addresses three main questions: What exactly are we aware of when making a movement? What is the contribution of afferent and efferent signals to motor awareness? What are the neural bases of motor awareness? It reviews evidence that the motor system is mainly aware of its intention. As long as the goal is achieved, nothing reaches awareness about the kinematic details of the ongoing movements, even when substantial corrections have to be implemented to attain the intended state. The chapter also shows that motor awareness relies mainly on the central predictive computations carried out within the posterior parietal cortex. The outcome of these computations is contrasted with the peripheral reafferent input to build a veridical motor awareness. Some evidence exists that this process involves the premotor areas

    Automatic correction of hand pointing in stereoscopic depth

    Get PDF
    In order to examine whether stereoscopic depth information could drive fast automatic correction of hand pointing, an experiment was designed in a 3D visual environment in which participants were asked to point to a target at different stereoscopic depths as quickly and accurately as possible within a limited time window (≀300 ms). The experiment consisted of two tasks: "depthGO" in which participants were asked to point to the new target position if the target jumped, and "depthSTOP" in which participants were instructed to abort their ongoing movements after the target jumped. The depth jump was designed to occur in 20% of the trials in both tasks. Results showed that fast automatic correction of hand movements could be driven by stereoscopic depth to occur in as early as 190 ms.This work was supported by the Grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (60970062 and 61173116) and the Doctoral Fund of Ministry of Education of China (20110072110014)

    On-line motor control in patients with Parkinson's disease

    Get PDF
    Recent models based, in part on a study of Huntington's disease, suggest that the basal ganglia are involved in on-line movement guidance. Two experiments were conducted to investigate this idea. First, we studied advanced Parkinson's disease patients performing a reaching task known to depend on on-line guidance. The task was to ‘look and point' in the dark at visual targets displayed in the peripheral visual field. In some trials, the target location was slightly modified during saccadic gaze displacement (when vision is suppressed). In both patient and control groups, the target jump induced a gradual modification of the movement which diverged smoothly from its original path to reach the new target location. No deficit was found in the patients, except for an increased latency to respond to the target jump (Parkinson's disease: 243 ms; controls: 166 ms). A computational simulation indicated that this response slowing was likely to be a by-product of bradykinesia. The unexpected inconsistency between this result and previous reports was investigated in a second experiment. We hypothesized that the relevant factor was the characteristics of the corrections to be performed. To test this prediction, we investigated a task requiring corrections of the same type as investigated in Huntington's disease, namely large, consciously detected errors induced by large target jumps at hand movement onset. In contrast with the smooth adjustments observed in the first experiment, the subjects responded to the target jump by generating a discrete corrective sub-movement. While this iterative response was relatively rapid in the control subjects (220 ms), Parkinson's disease patients exhibited either dramatically late (>730 ms) or totally absent on-line corrections. When on-line corrections were absent, the initial motor response was completed before a second corrective response was initiated (the latency of the corrective response was the same as the latency of the initial response). Considered together, these results suggest that basal ganglia dependent circuits are not critical for feedback loops involving a smooth modulation of the ongoing command. These circuits may rather contribute to the generation of discrete corrective sub-movements. This deficit is in line with the general impairment of sequential and simultaneous actions in patients with basal ganglia disorder

    Forward estimation of movement state in posterior parietal cortex

    Get PDF
    During goal-directed movements, primates are able to rapidly and accurately control an online trajectory despite substantial delay times incurred in the sensorimotor control loop. To address the problem of large delays, it has been proposed that the brain uses an internal forward model of the arm to estimate current and upcoming states of a movement, which are more useful for rapid online control. To study online control mechanisms in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC), we recorded from single neurons while monkeys performed a joystick task. Neurons encoded the static target direction and the dynamic movement angle of the cursor. The dynamic encoding properties of many movement angle neurons reflected a forward estimate of the state of the cursor that is neither directly available from passive sensory feedback nor compatible with outgoing motor commands and is consistent with PPC serving as a forward model for online sensorimotor control. In addition, we found that the space–time tuning functions of these neurons were largely separable in the angle–time plane, suggesting that they mostly encode straight and approximately instantaneous trajectories

    Dissociable contribution of the parietal and frontal cortex to coding movement direction and amplitude

    Get PDF
    © 2015 Davare, ZĂ©non, Desmurget and Olivier. To reach for an object, we must convert its spatial location into an appropriate motor command, merging movement direction and amplitude. In humans, it has been suggested that this visuo-motor transformation occurs in a dorsomedial parieto-frontal pathway, although the causal contribution of the areas constituting the “reaching circuit” remains unknown. Here we used transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in healthy volunteers to disrupt the function of either the medial intraparietal area (mIPS) or dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), in each hemisphere. The task consisted in performing step-tracking movements with the right wrist towards targets located in different directions and eccentricities; targets were either visible for the whole trial (Target-ON) or flashed for 200 ms (Target-OFF). Left and right mIPS disruption led to errors in the initial direction of movements performed towards contralateral targets. These errors were corrected online in the Target-ON condition but when the target was flashed for 200 ms, mIPS TMS manifested as a larger endpoint spreading. In contrast, left PMd virtual lesions led to higher acceleration and velocity peaks—two parameters typically used to probe the planned movement amplitude—irrespective of the target position, hemifield and presentation condition; in the Target-OFF condition, left PMd TMS induced overshooting and increased the endpoint dispersion along the axis of the target direction. These results indicate that left PMd intervenes in coding amplitude during movement preparation. The critical TMS timings leading to errors in direction and amplitude were different, namely 160–100 ms before movement onset for mIPS and 100–40 ms for left PMd. TMS applied over right PMd had no significant effect. These results demonstrate that, during motor preparation, direction and amplitude of goal-directed movements are processed by different cortical areas, at distinct timings, and according to a specific hemispheric organization.ARC (Actions de Recherche ConcertĂ©es, CommunautĂ© Française de Belgique); Fondation MĂ©dicale Reine Elisabeth (FMRE) and from the Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique (FNRS–FDP); BBSRC David Phillips fellowship (UK), the Royal Society (UK); FWO Odysseus project (Fonds WetenschappelijkOnderzoek,Belgium).AZisaSeniorResearch AssociatesupportedbyINNOVIRIS
    • 

    corecore