939 research outputs found
There is no special problem of mindreading in nonhuman animals
There is currently a consensus among comparative psychologists that nonhuman animals are capable of some forms of mindreading. Several philosophers and psychologists have criticized this consensus, however, arguing that there is a “logical problem” with the experimental approach used to test for mindreading in nonhuman animals. I argue that the logical problem is no more than a version of the general skeptical problem known as the theoretician’s dilemma. As such, it is not a problem that comparative psychologists must solve before providing evidence for mindreading.This is the final version of the article. It first appeared from The University of Chicago Press via http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/68162
Not null enough: pseudo-null hypotheses in community ecology and comparative psychology
We evaluate a common reasoning strategy used in community ecology and comparative psychology for selecting between competing hypotheses. This strategy labels one hypothesis as a “null” on the grounds of its simplicity and epistemically privileges it as accepted until rejected. We argue that this strategy is unjustified. The asymmetrical treatment of statistical null hypotheses is justified through the experimental and mathematical contexts in which they are used, but these contexts are missing in the case of the “pseudo-null hypotheses” found in our case studies. Moreover, statistical nulls are often not epistemically privileged in practice over their alternatives because failing to reject the null is usually a negative result about the alternative, experimental hypothesis. Scientists should eschew the appeal to pseudo-nulls. It is a rhetorical strategy that glosses over a commitment to valuing simplicity over other epistemic virtues in the name of good scientific and statistical methodology.Leverhulme Centre for the Future of Intelligenc
PARAMETRIC ESTIMATION OF HAZARD FUNCTIONS WITH STOCHASTIC COVARIATE PROCESSES
Let X(t), t âÂÂ¥ 0, be a real or vector valued stochastic process and T a
random killing-time of the process which generally depends on the sample function. In the
context of survival analysis, T represents the time to a prescribed event (e.g. system failure,
time of disease symptom, etc.) and X(t) is a stochastic covariate process, observed up to
time T. The conditional distribution of T, given X(t), t âÂÂ¥ 0, is assumed to be of a known
functional form with an unknown vector parameter ø; however, the distributions of X(â¢) are
not specified. For an arbitrary fixed ñ > 0 the observable data from a single realization of T
and X(â¢) is min(T, ñ), X(t), 0 ⤠t ⤠min(T, ñ). For n âÂÂ¥ 1 the maximum likelihood estimator
of ø is based on n independent copies of the observable data. It is shown that solutions of
the likelihood equation are consistent and asymptotically normal and efficient under specified
regularity conditions on the hazard function associated with the conditional distribution of T.
The Fisher information matrix is represented in terms of the hazard function. The form of the
hazard function is very general, and is not restricted to the commonly considered cases where
it depends on X(â¢) only through the present point X(t). Furthermore, the process X(â¢) is a
general, not necessarily Markovian process.Statistics Working Papers Serie
The goal of ape pointing
Captive great apes regularly use pointing gestures in their interactions with humans. However, the precise function of this gesture is unknown. One possibility is that apes use pointing primarily to direct attention (as in “please look at that”); another is that they point mainly as an action request (such as “can you give that to me?”). We investigated these two possibilities here by examining how the looking behavior of recipients affects pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Upon pointing to food, subjects were faced with a recipient who either looked at the indicated object (successful-look) or failed to look at the indicated object (failed-look). We predicted that, if apes point primarily to direct attention, subjects would spend more time pointing in the failed-look condition because the goal of their gesture had not been met. Alternatively, we expected that, if apes point primarily to request an object, subjects would not differ in their pointing behavior between the successful-look and failed-look conditions because these conditions differed only in the looking behavior of the recipient. We found that subjects did differ in their pointing behavior across the successful-look and failed-look conditions, but contrary to our prediction subjects spent more time pointing in the successful-look condition. These results suggest that apes are sensitive to the attentional states of gestural recipients, but their adjustments are aimed at multiple goals. We also found a greater number of individuals with a strong right-hand than left-hand preference for pointing
Xanthurenic acid translocates proapoptotic Bcl-2 family proteins into mitochondria and impairs mitochondrial function
BACKGROUND: Xanthurenic acid is an endogenous molecule produced by tryptophan degradation, produced in the cytoplasm and mitochondria. Its accumulation can be observed in aging-related diseases, e.g. senile cataract and infectious disease. We previously reported that xanthurenic acid provokes apoptosis, and now present a study of the response of mitochondria to xanthurenic acid. RESULTS: Xanthurenic acid at 10 or 20 μM in culture media of human aortic smooth muscle cells induces translocation of the proteins Bax, Bak, Bclx(s), and Bad into mitochondria. In 20 μM xanthurenic acid, Bax is also translocated to the nucleus. In isolated mitochondria xanthurenic acid leads to Bax and Bclx(s )oligomerization, accumulation of Ca(2+), and increased oxygen consumption. CONCLUSION: Xanthurenic acid interacts directly with Bcl-2 family proteins, inducing mitochondrial pathways of apoptosis and impairing mitochondrial functions
Why 1,2‑quinone derivatives are more stable than their 2,3‑analogues?
In this work, we have studied the relative stability
of 1,2- and 2,3-quinones. While 1,2-quinones have
a closed-shell singlet ground state, the ground state for
the studied 2,3-isomers is open-shell singlet, except for
2,3-naphthaquinone that has a closed-shell singlet ground
state. In all cases, 1,2-quinones are more stable than their
2,3-counterparts. We analyzed the reasons for the higher
stability of the 1,2-isomers through energy decomposition
analysis in the framework of Kohn–Sham molecular orbital
theory. The results showed that we have to trace the origin
of 1,2-quinones’ enhanced stability to the more efficient
bonding in the π-electron system due to more favorable
overlap between the SOMOπ of the ·C4n−2H2n–CH·· and
··CH–CO–CO· fragments in the 1,2-arrangement. Furthermore,
whereas 1,2-quinones present a constant trend with their elongation for all analyzed properties (geometric,
energetic, and electronic), 2,3-quinone derivatives present a
substantial breaking in monotonicity.European
Union in the framework of European Social Fund through the Warsaw
University of Technology Development Programme. O.A. S., H.
S. and T.M. K
Modelling optical micro-machines
A strongly focused laser beam can be used to trap, manipulate and exert
torque on a microparticle. The torque is the result of transfer of angular
momentum by scattering of the laser beam. The laser could be used to drive a
rotor, impeller, cog wheel or some other microdevice of a few microns in size,
perhaps fabricated from a birefringent material. We review our methods of
computationally simulating the torque and force imparted by a laser beam. We
introduce a method of hybridizing the T-matrix with the Finite Difference
Frequency Domain (FDFD) method to allow the modelling of materials that are
anisotropic and inhomogeneous, and structures that have complex shapes. The
high degree of symmetry of a microrotor, such as discrete or continuous
rotational symmetry, can be exploited to reduce computational time and memory
requirements by orders of magnitude. This is achieved by performing
calculations for only a given segment or plane that is repeated across the
whole structure. This can be demonstrated by modelling the optical trapping and
rotation of a cube.Comment: 4 pages, 3 figure
Roadmap on structured light
Structured light refers to the generation and application of custom light fields. As the tools and technology to create and detect structured light have evolved, steadily the applications have begun to emerge. This roadmap touches on the key fields within structured light from the perspective of experts in those areas, providing insight into the current state and the challenges their respective fields face. Collectively the roadmap outlines the venerable nature of structured light research and the exciting prospects for the future that are yet to be realized.Peer ReviewedPostprint (published version
- …