82 research outputs found

    Overcoming perceptual features in logical reasoning: an event-related potentials study

    Get PDF
    It is more difficult for reasoners to detect that the letter-number pair H7 verifies the conditional rule If there is not a T then there is not a 4 than to detect that it verifies the rule If there is an H then there is a 7. In prior work [Prado, J., & Noveck, I. A. (2007). Overcoming perceptual features in logical reasoning: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(4), 642-657], we argued that this difficulty was due to mismatching effects, i.e. perceptual mismatches that arise when the items mentioned in the rule (e.g. T and 4) mismatch those presented in the test-pair (H and 7). The present study aimed to test this claim directly by recording ERPs while participants evaluated conditional rules in the presence or absence of mismatches. We found that mismatches, not only trigger a frontocentral N2 (an ERP known to be related to perceptual mismatch) but that they, parametrically modulate its amplitude (e.g. two mismatches prompt a greater N2 than one). Our results indicate that the main role of negations in conditional rules is to focus attention on the negated constituent but also suggest that there is some inter-individual differences in the way participants apprehend such negations, as indicated by a correlation between N2 amplitude and participants' reaction times. Overall, these findings emphasize how overcoming perceptual features plays a role in the mismatching effect and extend the mismatch-related effects of the N2 into a reasoning task

    Action relevance in linguistic context drives word-induced motor activity

    Get PDF
    Many neurocognitive studies on the role of motor structures in action-language processing have implicitly adopted a “dictionary-like” framework within which lexical meaning is constructed on the basis of an invariant set of semantic features. The debate has thus been centered on the question of whether motor activation is an integral part of the lexical semantics (embodied theories) or the result of a post-lexical construction of a situation model (disembodied theories). However, research in psycholinguistics show that lexical semantic processing and context-dependent meaning construction are narrowly integrated. An understanding of the role of motor structures in action-language processing might thus be better achieved by focusing on the linguistic contexts under which such structures are recruited. Here, we therefore analyzed online modulations of grip force while subjects listened to target words embedded in different linguistic contexts. When the target word was a hand action verb and when the sentence focused on that action (John signs the contract) an early increase of grip force was observed. No comparable increase was detected when the same word occurred in a context that shifted the focus toward the agent's mental state (John wants to sign the contract). There mere presence of an action word is thus not sufficient to trigger motor activation. Moreover, when the linguistic context set up a strong expectation for a hand action, a grip force increase was observed even when the tested word was a pseudo-verb. The presence of a known action word is thus not required to trigger motor activation. Importantly, however, the same linguistic contexts that sufficed to trigger motor activation with pseudo-verbs failed to trigger motor activation when the target words were verbs with no motor action reference. Context is thus not by itself sufficient to supersede an “incompatible” word meaning. We argue that motor structure activation is part of a dynamic process that integrates the lexical meaning potential of a term and the context in the online construction of a situation model, which is a crucial process for fluent and efficient online language comprehension

    Grip Force Reveals the Context Sensitivity of Language-Induced Motor Activity during “Action Words

    Get PDF
    Studies demonstrating the involvement of motor brain structures in language processing typically focus on \ud time windows beyond the latencies of lexical-semantic access. Consequently, such studies remain inconclusive regarding whether motor brain structures are recruited directly in language processing or through post-linguistic conceptual imagery. In the present study, we introduce a grip-force sensor that allows online measurements of language-induced motor activity during sentence listening. We use this tool to investigate whether language-induced motor activity remains constant or is modulated in negative, as opposed to affirmative, linguistic contexts. Our findings demonstrate that this simple experimental paradigm can be used to study the online crosstalk between language and the motor systems in an ecological and economical manner. Our data further confirm that the motor brain structures that can be called upon during action word processing are not mandatorily involved; the crosstalk is asymmetrically\ud governed by the linguistic context and not vice versa

    Méthodes de localisation des générateurs de l’activité électrique cérébrale à partir de signaux électro- et magnéto-encéphalographiques.

    No full text
    This work presents numerical evaluations of several dipole localization approaches from electroencephalographic (EEG) and magnetoencephalographic (MEG) recordings. First of all, the intrinsic accuracy of realistic modeling with the boundary element method (collocation) was systematically evaluated. Using a linear interpolation of the potential on each mesh triangle rather than a constant interpolation provided only a slight improvement of the EEG inverse problem with the same computing cost. Next, the localization bias introduced by the classical spherical model was then quantified. This bias was found to range from 2.5 mm in the upper part of the head to 8 mm in the lower part of the head. Then it was shown that, contrarily to spherical models, realistic models could retrieve dipole orientation with less than 20◦ error, even for radial orientations and noisy data. Several techniques for combining EEG and MEG in a single inverse problem were finally evaluated on simulated data with spatially correlated noise. All these coupling techniques were found to provide a better or equal localization accuracy compared to the best of either modality, even when using few electrodes. Combining MEG and EEG with a realistic model and the boundary element method thus produces a robust method for localizing brain electrical activity.Cette thèse présente des évaluations par simulation numérique de plusieurs approches de localisation des générateurs de l’activité électrique cérébrale modélisés par des dipôles de courant équivalents à partir de données électroencéphalographiques (EEG) et magnétoencéphalographiques (MEG). Nous avons tout d’abord évalué systématiquement la précision intrinsèque des modèles à géométrie réaliste avec la méthode des intégrales de surface (collocation). Nous avons montré que l’utilisation d’une interpolation linéaire plutôt que constante du potentiel sur chaque triangle du maillage ne permettait qu’une légère amélioration de la précision lors de la résolution du problème direct EEG pour un temps de calcul identique. Ensuite, nous avons quantifié le biais de localisation introduit par l’utilisation du modèle sphérique classique en MEG. Nous avons trouvé un biais allant de 2.5 mm en haut de la tête à 8 mm en bas de la tête. Nous avons également mis en évidence que les modèles réalistes, contrairement aux modèles sphériques, permettent de retrouver l’orientation des sources (avec une erreur inférieure à 20◦) même lorsqu’elles sont radiales et même en présence de bruit. Enfin, différentes méthodes de couplage des données MEG et EEG dans un même problème inverse ont également été évaluées sur des données simulées avec un bruit réaliste corrélé spatialement. Nous avons montré que ce couplage permettait d’aboutir à une précision supérieure ou égale à la meilleure des deux modalités, et ce même avec un petit nombre d’électrodes. Le couplage MEG/EEG dans un modèle réaliste avec la méthode des intégrales de surface permet donc une localisation plus robuste de l’activité électrique cérébrale

    Méthodes de localisation des générateurs de l’activité électrique cérébrale à partir de signaux électro- et magnéto-encéphalographiques.

    No full text
    This work presents numerical evaluations of several dipole localization approaches from electroencephalographic (EEG) and magnetoencephalographic (MEG) recordings. First of all, the intrinsic accuracy of realistic modeling with the boundary element method (collocation) was systematically evaluated. Using a linear interpolation of the potential on each mesh triangle rather than a constant interpolation provided only a slight improvement of the EEG inverse problem with the same computing cost. Next, the localization bias introduced by the classical spherical model was then quantified. This bias was found to range from 2.5 mm in the upper part of the head to 8 mm in the lower part of the head. Then it was shown that, contrarily to spherical models, realistic models could retrieve dipole orientation with less than 20◦ error, even for radial orientations and noisy data. Several techniques for combining EEG and MEG in a single inverse problem were finally evaluated on simulated data with spatially correlated noise. All these coupling techniques were found to provide a better or equal localization accuracy compared to the best of either modality, even when using few electrodes. Combining MEG and EEG with a realistic model and the boundary element method thus produces a robust method for localizing brain electrical activity.Cette thèse présente des évaluations par simulation numérique de plusieurs approches de localisation des générateurs de l’activité électrique cérébrale modélisés par des dipôles de courant équivalents à partir de données électroencéphalographiques (EEG) et magnétoencéphalographiques (MEG). Nous avons tout d’abord évalué systématiquement la précision intrinsèque des modèles à géométrie réaliste avec la méthode des intégrales de surface (collocation). Nous avons montré que l’utilisation d’une interpolation linéaire plutôt que constante du potentiel sur chaque triangle du maillage ne permettait qu’une légère amélioration de la précision lors de la résolution du problème direct EEG pour un temps de calcul identique. Ensuite, nous avons quantifié le biais de localisation introduit par l’utilisation du modèle sphérique classique en MEG. Nous avons trouvé un biais allant de 2.5 mm en haut de la tête à 8 mm en bas de la tête. Nous avons également mis en évidence que les modèles réalistes, contrairement aux modèles sphériques, permettent de retrouver l’orientation des sources (avec une erreur inférieure à 20◦) même lorsqu’elles sont radiales et même en présence de bruit. Enfin, différentes méthodes de couplage des données MEG et EEG dans un même problème inverse ont également été évaluées sur des données simulées avec un bruit réaliste corrélé spatialement. Nous avons montré que ce couplage permettait d’aboutir à une précision supérieure ou égale à la meilleure des deux modalités, et ce même avec un petit nombre d’électrodes. Le couplage MEG/EEG dans un modèle réaliste avec la méthode des intégrales de surface permet donc une localisation plus robuste de l’activité électrique cérébrale

    Double Negation in a Negative Concord language : An experimental investigation

    Get PDF
    This paper investigates the interpretation and processing of simple transitive Catalan sentences with multiple negative expressions experimentally. Our results provide empirical confirmation that Negative Concord (NC) is the preferred and faster interpretation for negative sentences that either omit or contain the overt negative marker no 'not'. However, they also reveal that, in contrast to traditional descriptions of Catalan and independently of particular favouring contexts, a non-negligible amount of Double Negation (DN) readings arises, mainly when the negative marker co-occurs with pre-verbal Negative Concord Items (NCIs), and when these NCIs have a complex DP structure. Our results further suggest that two populations could be distinguished: one for whom the negative marker is optional and leaves the favoured NC reading essentially unaffected, and another where the co-presence of no significantly increases DN readings. We account for these findings within a micro-parametric approach that features ambiguous NCIs (non-negative vs. negative) and a possible ambiguous negative marker no (negative vs. expletive) variably available for Catalan speakers. The nuanced empirical NC landscape that our experimental work reveals serves to stress the importance of taking DN readings into consideration for a better understanding of the nature of negative constructions in Catalan and cross-linguistically

    Méthodes de localisation des générateurs de l'activité électrique cérébrale à partir de signaux électro-et magnéto-encéphalographiques

    No full text
    Cette thèse présente des évaluations par simulation numérique de plusieurs approches de localisation des générateurs de l'activité électrique cérébrale modélises par des dipôles de courant équivalents a partir de données électro-encéphalographiques (EEG) et magnéto-encéphalographiques (MEG). Nous avons tout d'abord évalue systématiquement la précision intrinsèques des modèles a géométrie réaliste avec la méthode des intégrales de surface (collocation). Nous avons montre que l'utilisation d'une interpolation linéaire plutôt que constante du potentiel sur chaque triangle du maillage ne permettait qu'une légère amélioration de la précision lors de la résolution du problème direct EEG pour un temps de calcul identique. Ensuite, nous avons quantifie le biais de localisation introduit par l'utilisation du modèle sphérique classique en MEG. Nous avons trouve un biais allant de 2.5 mm en haut de la tête a 8 mm en bas de la tête. Nous avons également mis en évidence que les modèles réalistes, contrairement aux modèles sphériques, - permettent de retrouver l'orientation des sources (avec une erreur inferieure a 20 degrés) même lorsqu'elles sont radiales et même en présence de bruit. Enfin, différentes méthodes de couplage des données MEG et EEG dans un même problème inverse ont également été évaluées sur des données simulées avec un bruit réaliste corrèle spatialement. Nous avons montre que ce couplage permettait d'aboutir a une précision supérieure ou égale a la meilleure des deux modalités, et ce même avec un petit nombre d'électrodes. Le couplage MEG/EEG dans un modèle réaliste avec la méthode des intégrales de surface permet donc une localisation plus robuste de l'activité électrique cérébrale.This work presents numerical evaluations of several dipole localization approaches from electroencephalographic (EEG) and magnetoencephalographic (MEG) recordings. First of all, the intrinsic accuracy of realistic modeling with the boundary element method (collocation) was systematically evaluated. Using a linear interpolation of the potential on each mesh triangle rather than a constant interpolation provided only a slight improvement of the EEG inverse problem with the same computing cost. Next, the localization bias introduced by the classical spherical model was then quantified. This bias was found to range from 2.5 mm in the upper part of the head to 8 mm in the lower part of the head. Then it was shown that, contrarily to spherical models, realistic models could retrieve dipole orientation with less than 20 degree error, even for radial orientations and noisy data. Several techniques for combining EEG and MEG in a single inverse problem were finally evaluated on simulated data with spatially correlated noise. All these coupling techniques were found to provide better or equal localization accuracy compared to the best of either modality, even when using few electrodes. Combining MEG and EEG with a realistic model and the boundary element method thus produces a robust method for localizing brain electrical activity.VILLEURBANNE-DOC'INSA LYON (692662301) / SudocSudocFranceF

    When and How is Concord preferred? An Experimental approach

    No full text
    ISBN : 978-2-8399-1580-9.International audienceA longstanding debate asks whether negative polarity (1a.NPI) and negative concord (1b.NC) involve identical or distinct syntactic/semantic operations. Although French (1a&b) and cross-linguistic equivalents share the same first-order-logic interpretation (2a), disagreements remain as to how it obtains for each. Yet only (1b), ambiguously allows a double negative (DN)(2b). Taking the English paraphrase in (2b) to be likewise ambiguous, May (89) proposed that DN encodes a compositional hierarchical scope relation between its negative quantifiers (3a), while NC involves the formation of a resumptive polyadic negative quantifier (3b). Applied to French, this analysis of NC has long ranging consequences. First, NC is clearly distinguished from NPI, as n-words are cast as negative quantifiers. Second, it puts French (1b) and English (2b) under the same theoretical umbrella, questioning the validity of any NC macro-parameter. Third, how (1b) and (2b) should be distinguished arises anew, particularly if French truly favors NC, but English DN. Indeed, although the analysis elegantly allows both readings for (1b)-(2b) without lexical ambiguity, it remains surprisingly vague as to which factors favors NC over DN. Processing costs, intonation, quantifier parallelism, structural complexity, clause boundedness and discourse have all been suggested to influence DN or NC, but in effect, little is known as to how speakers resolve such ambiguities in a single language. The paper explores this question experimentally. Ambiguous sentences like (1b) were paired with two scenes, each representing one reading. Subjects were asked to read them aloud and pick the one that representing its meaning. Sound production and choice time through mouse tracking were recorded. Quantifier parallelism, structural complexity and syntactic position were manipulated to probe their effects on interpretation. The design produces experimental data on the effects of quantificational parallelism, structural complexity, syntactic position, and processing time for NC vs. DN and intonation. On a resumptive quantification analysis of NC, theoretical predictions are as follows: A) parallel simple Pro-Pro structures should produce stronger NC preference than more complex NP-NP ones; non-parallel structures should favor DN. B) DN readings should lead lengthened choice time vs NC C) NC and DN preference should manifest characteristically distinct intonation contours. The paper reports on the first results of this experiment. (1)a. Personne ne fait quoique ce soit. b. Personne ne fait rien. (2)a ¬∃x ∃y do(x,y) ‘No one does anything’. b.¬∃x,¬∃y do(x,y) ‘No one does nothing’. (3)a.(NOx (NOy do(x,y))) b.NO do(x,y

    When and How is Concord preferred? An Experimental approach

    No full text
    ISBN : 978-2-8399-1580-9.International audienceA longstanding debate asks whether negative polarity (1a.NPI) and negative concord (1b.NC) involve identical or distinct syntactic/semantic operations. Although French (1a&b) and cross-linguistic equivalents share the same first-order-logic interpretation (2a), disagreements remain as to how it obtains for each. Yet only (1b), ambiguously allows a double negative (DN)(2b). Taking the English paraphrase in (2b) to be likewise ambiguous, May (89) proposed that DN encodes a compositional hierarchical scope relation between its negative quantifiers (3a), while NC involves the formation of a resumptive polyadic negative quantifier (3b). Applied to French, this analysis of NC has long ranging consequences. First, NC is clearly distinguished from NPI, as n-words are cast as negative quantifiers. Second, it puts French (1b) and English (2b) under the same theoretical umbrella, questioning the validity of any NC macro-parameter. Third, how (1b) and (2b) should be distinguished arises anew, particularly if French truly favors NC, but English DN. Indeed, although the analysis elegantly allows both readings for (1b)-(2b) without lexical ambiguity, it remains surprisingly vague as to which factors favors NC over DN. Processing costs, intonation, quantifier parallelism, structural complexity, clause boundedness and discourse have all been suggested to influence DN or NC, but in effect, little is known as to how speakers resolve such ambiguities in a single language. The paper explores this question experimentally. Ambiguous sentences like (1b) were paired with two scenes, each representing one reading. Subjects were asked to read them aloud and pick the one that representing its meaning. Sound production and choice time through mouse tracking were recorded. Quantifier parallelism, structural complexity and syntactic position were manipulated to probe their effects on interpretation. The design produces experimental data on the effects of quantificational parallelism, structural complexity, syntactic position, and processing time for NC vs. DN and intonation. On a resumptive quantification analysis of NC, theoretical predictions are as follows: A) parallel simple Pro-Pro structures should produce stronger NC preference than more complex NP-NP ones; non-parallel structures should favor DN. B) DN readings should lead lengthened choice time vs NC C) NC and DN preference should manifest characteristically distinct intonation contours. The paper reports on the first results of this experiment. (1)a. Personne ne fait quoique ce soit. b. Personne ne fait rien. (2)a ¬∃x ∃y do(x,y) ‘No one does anything’. b.¬∃x,¬∃y do(x,y) ‘No one does nothing’. (3)a.(NOx (NOy do(x,y))) b.NO do(x,y
    corecore