676 research outputs found
Process Capability Database Usage In Industry: Myth vs. Reality
Process capability data (PCD) is needed for robust design, optimal tolerance allocation, and variation
simulation analysis. Process capability databases (PCDBs) have been developed in many industries and are
being used by the manufacturing community to monitor quality; however, they are not being effectively
utilized by design. When the PCDBs1 were developed, the intent was for design to use PCD for
optimization and product cost minimization, but this ideal situation has not been realized.
A survey of a variety of design and manufacturing companies was circulated to determine both the state-ofthe-
art in PCDBs and the barriers preventing design from fully utilizing PCD. Two key barriers were
identified for internal PCDBs: lack of a company-wide vision for PCD usage and poor communication
between manufacturing and design. Supplier PCDBs have the additional barriers of lack of trust between
suppliers and customers and time lag for data entry. Management support, training, database population,
and common systems were identified as potential solutions to the identified barriers
Tube Production and Assembly Systems: The Impact of Compliance and Variability on Yield
Variation modeling is used in design to predict and
diagnose potential quality problems. Most variation modeling
assumes the parts are rigidly assembled. However, in some
cases, this assumption is invalid. For example, when hydraulic
tubes are assembled into aircraft structures, the compliance of
the tube facilitates assembly. If the tubes were rigid, they
cannot be assembled, i.e., the variations of the tubes and
structures are too great. Despite the importance of compliance
in assembly, it is typically not explicitly modeled during
design. This paper proposes a new method to directly predict
the assemblability of any tube design with minimal dependence
CAD/FEM modeling and simulation. The model includes a
variation model for the tubes and aircraft, compliance model
and assembly model. It can be used during design to improve
yields
Archeota, Fall 2018
This is the Fall 2018 issue of Archeota, the official publication of the SJSU SAASChttps://scholarworks.sjsu.edu/saasc_archeota/1008/thumbnail.jp
Two Negations for the Price of One
Standard English is typically described as a double negation language. In double negation Âlanguages, each negative marker contributes independent semantic force. Two negations in the same clause usually cancel each other out, resulting in an affirmative sentence. Other dialects of English permit negative concord. In negative concord sentences, the two negative markers yield a single semantic negation. This paper explores how English-speaking children interpret sentences with more than one negative element, in order to assess whether their early grammar allows negative concord. According to Zeijlstra’s (2004) typological generalization, if a language has a negative syntactic head, it will be a negative concord language. Since Standard English is often analysed as having a negative head, it represents an apparent exception to Zeijlstra’s Âgeneralization. This raises the intriguing possibility that initially, children recognize that English has a negative head (i.e., 'n’t') and, therefore, assign negative concord interpretations to sentences with two negations, despite the absence of evidence for this interpretation in the adult input. The present study investigated this possibility in a comprehension study with 20 3- to 5-year-old Âchildren and a control group of 15 adults. The test sentences were presented in contexts that made them amenable to either a double negation or a negative concord interpretation. As expected, the adult participants assigned the double negation interpretation of the test sentences the majority of the time. In contrast, the child participants assigned the alternative, negative concord interpretation the majority of the time. Children must jettison the negative concord interpretation of sentences with two negative markers, and acquire a double negation interpretation. We propose that the requisite positive evidence is the appearance of negative expressions like 'nothing 'in object position. Because such expressions exert semantic force without a second negation, this informs children that they are acquiring a double negation language
The acquisition of questions with long-distance dependencies
A number of researchers have claimed that questions and other constructions with long distance dependencies (LDDs) are acquired relatively early, by age 4 or even earlier, in spite of their complexity. Analysis of LDD questions in the input available to children suggests that they are extremely stereotypical, raising the possibility that children learn lexically specific templates such as WH do you think S-GAP? rather than general rules of the kind postulated in traditional linguistic accounts of this construction. We describe three elicited imitation experiments with children aged from 4;6 to 6;9 and adult controls. Participants were asked to repeat prototypical questions (i.e., questions which match the hypothesised template), unprototypical questions (which depart from it in several respects) and declarative counterparts of both types of interrogative sentences. The children performed significantly better on the prototypical variants of both constructions, even when both variants contained exactly the same lexical material, while adults showed prototypicality e¤ects for LDD questions only. These results suggest that a general declarative complementation construction emerges quite late in development (after age 6), and that even adults rely on lexically specific templates for LDD questions
Archeota, Spring 2019
This is the Spring 2019 issue of Archeota, the official publication of SJSU SAASC.
Archeota is a platform for students to contribute to the archival conversation. It is written BY students, FOR students. It provides substantive content on archival concerns and issues, and promotes career development in the field of archival studies. Archeota upholds the core values of the archival profession. It is a semiannual publication of the Student Chapter of the Society of American Archivists at the San Jose State University School of Information.https://scholarworks.sjsu.edu/saasc_archeota/1009/thumbnail.jp
The effect of using a cycling workstation on mouse dexterity
This study investigated the effect of using a cycling workstation on mouse dexterity, including if and how this changed with practice. Thirty-four healthy adults were allocated to a sitting group (n = 17) or cycling group (n = 17). All participants completed standardised computer tasks on 6 occasions: baseline and final—all participants were seated; practice 1 to 4—sitting group participants were seated, cycling group participants pedalled on an under desk cycle. Three computer tasks were employed: (1) Tracking (continuous task)—participants used the mouse pointer to track a dot in a figure of 8 pattern at 3 different speeds without a guide then with a guide (2) Aiming (discrete task)—participants moved the mouse pointer to a dot which repeatedly disappeared then reappeared again in different locations, creating the outline of a pentagram (3) Steering (continuous task)—participants steered the mouse pointer around two different pathways. Accuracy was measured during the Tracking and Steering tasks as the root mean square error and penalised path accuracy respectively. Speed was measured during the Aiming task as the movement time. Data was analysed using frequentist and Bayes Factor analyses. During the continuous tasks (Tracking and Steering), accuracy was impaired among participants using the cycling workstation, both compared to their accuracy when seated and to the accuracy of participants in the sitting group. In contrast, no deficits in speed were noted among participants using the cycling work station during the discrete task (Aiming). No learning effects were observed among either group for any tasks. These findings suggest using a cycling workstation may impair the accuracy but not speed of mouse use, regardless of task practice. Overall this supports the implementation of cycling workstations in typical office settings, but suggests cycling workstations may impair productivity among workers performing high precision mouse tasks
The effectiveness and safety of complementary health approaches to managing postpartum pain : a systematic review and meta-analysis
Background: Women experience pain from a number of causes during the postpartum period. Although pharmacological pain relief has shown to be effective, the efficacy of non-pharmacological methods of pain relief will be of interest to breastfeeding women. The aim of this systematic review was to examine the efficacy and safety of complementary approaches to manage postpartum pain. Methods: A search of English language databases from their inception to 2020 was undertaken for randomised controlled trials and included primiparous and multiparous women who experienced postpartum pain up to two weeks post birth. The primary outcome was pain. The risk of bias was assessed using the Cochrane risk of bias tool. Results. Thirty trials were included in the review, 25 trials (2,413 women) were included in the meta-analysis. Two trials of massage found a reduction in pain following caesarean birth within the first 24 hours post birth (MD -2.64, 95% -2.82 to -2.46, 184 women, I2 0%), and at seven days postpartum (MD -1.91, 95%CI -2.42 to -1.40, 2 trials, 120 women I2 37%). Two trials conducted with women receiving an episiotomy found reduction in perineal pain from herbal ointments within 24 hours (MD -1.33, 95% CI -.96 to -0.70, 221 women) and at 14 days postpartum (MD -0.74, 95% CI -1.02 to -0.47, 4 trials). Few trials reported on safety, few trials were at an overall low risk of bias, and overall the quality of evidence was very low. Conclusion. Further high quality trials are needed to determine the safety and effectiveness of herbal ointment and massage during the early postpartum period
- …