1,276 research outputs found
Percolation games, probabilistic cellular automata, and the hard-core model
Let each site of the square lattice be independently assigned
one of three states: a \textit{trap} with probability , a \textit{target}
with probability , and \textit{open} with probability , where
. Consider the following game: a token starts at the origin, and two
players take turns to move, where a move consists of moving the token from its
current site to either or . A player who moves the token
to a trap loses the game immediately, while a player who moves the token to a
target wins the game immediately. Is there positive probability that the game
is \emph{drawn} with best play -- i.e.\ that neither player can force a win?
This is equivalent to the question of ergodicity of a certain family of
elementary one-dimensional probabilistic cellular automata (PCA). These
automata have been studied in the contexts of enumeration of directed lattice
animals, the golden-mean subshift, and the hard-core model, and their
ergodicity has been noted as an open problem by several authors. We prove that
these PCA are ergodic, and correspondingly that the game on has
no draws.
On the other hand, we prove that certain analogous games \emph{do} exhibit
draws for suitable parameter values on various directed graphs in higher
dimensions, including an oriented version of the even sublattice of
in all . This is proved via a dimension reduction to a
hard-core lattice gas in dimension . We show that draws occur whenever the
corresponding hard-core model has multiple Gibbs distributions. We conjecture
that draws occur also on the standard oriented lattice for
, but here our method encounters a fundamental obstacle.Comment: 35 page
Self-management of recurrent headache
Na primeira parte desta revisão sobre terapêutica não-farmacológica das cefaléias, são discutidos os príncipios e a eficácia das principais formas de intervenção psicológica para enxaqueca recorrente e cefaléia tensional (técnicas de relaxamento ou de “biofeedback”e controle do estresse). Na segunda parte, são apresentados programas detalhados de treinamento de relaxamento ou de biofeedback pelo aquecimento das mãos. Finalmente, são discutidas brevemente os critérios para alterar ou terminar o tratamento.In this first part of this review of nonpharmacological therapies for headache, principles and efficacy of main categories of psychological interventions for recurrent migraine and tension-type headache (relaxation training, biofeedback training and stress management) are discussed. In the second part, detailed programs of relaxation training and handwarming biofeedback training are presented. Finally, criteria for altering or terminating treatment are briefly discussed
Percolation in invariant Poisson graphs with i.i.d. degrees
Let each point of a homogeneous Poisson process in R^d independently be
equipped with a random number of stubs (half-edges) according to a given
probability distribution mu on the positive integers. We consider
translation-invariant schemes for perfectly matching the stubs to obtain a
simple graph with degree distribution mu. Leaving aside degenerate cases, we
prove that for any mu there exist schemes that give only finite components as
well as schemes that give infinite components. For a particular matching scheme
that is a natural extension of Gale-Shapley stable marriage, we give sufficient
conditions on mu for the absence and presence of infinite components
Bootstrap Percolation on Complex Networks
We consider bootstrap percolation on uncorrelated complex networks. We obtain
the phase diagram for this process with respect to two parameters: , the
fraction of vertices initially activated, and , the fraction of undamaged
vertices in the graph. We observe two transitions: the giant active component
appears continuously at a first threshold. There may also be a second,
discontinuous, hybrid transition at a higher threshold. Avalanches of
activations increase in size as this second critical point is approached,
finally diverging at this threshold. We describe the existence of a special
critical point at which this second transition first appears. In networks with
degree distributions whose second moment diverges (but whose first moment does
not), we find a qualitatively different behavior. In this case the giant active
component appears for any and , and the discontinuous transition is
absent. This means that the giant active component is robust to damage, and
also is very easily activated. We also formulate a generalized bootstrap
process in which each vertex can have an arbitrary threshold.Comment: 9 pages, 3 figure
Finitary Coloring
Suppose that the vertices of are assigned random colors via a
finitary factor of independent identically distributed (iid) vertex-labels.
That is, the color of vertex is determined by a rule that examines the
labels within a finite (but random and perhaps unbounded) distance of ,
and the same rule applies at all vertices. We investigate the tail behavior of
if the coloring is required to be proper (that is, if adjacent vertices
must receive different colors). When , the optimal tail is given by a
power law for 3 colors, and a tower (iterated exponential) function for 4 or
more colors (and also for 3 or more colors when ). If proper coloring is
replaced with any shift of finite type in dimension 1, then, apart from trivial
cases, tower function behavior also applies.Comment: 35 pages, 3 figure
Neural dynamics of error processing in medial frontal cortex.
Contains fulltext :
56338.pdf (publisher's version ) (Closed access)Adaptive behavior requires an organism to evaluate the outcome of its actions, such that future behavior can be adjusted accordingly and the appropriate response selected. During associative learning, the time at which such evaluative information is available changes as learning progresses, from the delivery of performance feedback early in learning to the execution of the response itself during learned performance. Here, we report a learning-dependent shift in the timing of activation in the rostral cingulate zone of the anterior cingulate cortex from external error feedback to internal error detection. This pattern of activity is seen only in the anterior cingulate, not in the presupplementary motor area. The dynamics of these reciprocal changes are consistent with the claim that the rostral cingulate zone is involved in response selection on the basis of the expected outcome of an action. Specifically, these data illustrate how the anterior cingulate receives evaluative information, indicating that an action has not produced the desired result
- …