80 research outputs found

    The Neural Substrates of Subjective Time Dilation

    Get PDF
    An object moving towards an observer is subjectively perceived as longer in duration than the same object that is static or moving away. This ”time dilation effect” has been shown for a number of stimuli that differ from standard events along different feature dimensions (e.g. color, size, and dynamics). We performed an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, while subjects viewed a stream of five visual events, all of which were static and of identical duration except the fourth one, which was a deviant target consisting of either a looming or a receding disc. The duration of the target was systematically varied and participants judged whether the target was shorter or longer than all other events. A time dilation effect was observed only for looming targets. Relative to the static standards, the looming as well as the receding targets induced increased activation of the anterior insula and anterior cingulate cortices (the ”core control network”). The decisive contrast between looming and receding targets representing the time dilation effect showed strong asymmetric activation and, specifically, activation of cortical midline structures (the ”default network”). These results provide the first evidence that the illusion of temporal dilation is due to activation of areas that are important for cognitive control and subjective awareness. The involvement of midline structures in the temporal dilation illusion is interpreted as evidence that time perception is related to self-referential processing

    A volumetric comparison of the insular cortex and its subregions in primates

    Get PDF
    The neuronal composition of the insula in primates displays a gradient, transitioning from granular neocortex in the posterior-dorsal insula to agranular neocortex in the anterior-ventral insula with an intermediate zone of dysgranularity. Additionally, apes and humans exhibit a distinctive subdomain in the agranular insula, the frontoinsular cortex (FI), defined by the presence of clusters of von Economo neurons (VENs). Studies in humans indicate that the ventral anterior insula, including agranular insular cortex and FI, is involved in social awareness, and that the posterodorsal insula, including granular and dysgranular cortices, produces an internal representation of the body's homeostatic state. We examined the volumes of these cytoarchitectural areas of insular cortex in 30 primate species, including the volume of FI in apes and humans. Results indicate that the whole insula scales hyperallometrically (exponent = 1.13) relative to total brain mass, and the agranular insula (including FI) scales against total brain mass with even greater positive allometry (exponent = 1.23), providing a potential neural basis for enhancement of social cognition in association with increased brain size. The relative volumes of the subdivisions of the insular cortex, after controlling for total brain volume, are not correlated with species typical social group size. Although its size is predicted by primate-wide allometric scaling patterns, we found that the absolute volume of the left and right agranular insula and left FI are among the most differentially expanded of the human cerebral cortex compared to our closest living relative, the chimpanzee

    Damage to insula abolishes cognitive distortions during simulated gambling.

    Get PDF
    This is the accepted version of an article originally published in PNAS. The version of record is available at http://www.pnas.org/content/early/2014/04/02/1322295111.Gambling is a naturalistic example of risky decision-making. During gambling, players typically display an array of cognitive biases that create a distorted expectancy of winning. This study investigated brain regions underpinning gambling-related cognitive distortions, contrasting patients with focal brain lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), insula, or amygdala ("target patients") against healthy comparison participants and lesion comparison patients (i.e., with lesions that spare the target regions). A slot machine task was used to deliver near-miss outcomes (i.e., nonwins that fall spatially close to a jackpot), and a roulette game was used to examine the gambler's fallacy (color decisions following outcome runs). Comparison groups displayed a heightened motivation to play following near misses (compared with full misses), and manifested a classic gambler's fallacy effect. Both effects were also observed in patients with vmPFC and amygdala damage, but were absent in patients with insula damage. Our findings indicate that the distorted cognitive processing of near-miss outcomes and event sequences may be ordinarily supported by the recruitment of the insula. Interventions to reduce insula reactivity could show promise in the treatment of disordered gambling.LC was supported by a grant from the Medical Research Council (UK) (G1100554). BS was supported by a PhD studentship from the Medical Research Council. AB and DT, as well as the lesion patient research, were supported by grants from the National Institute of Health, namely the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke [P01 NS19632], and by the National Institute on Drug Abuse [R01 DA023051, R01 DA022549]

    The Relationship between Amygdala Activation and Passive Exposure Time to an Aversive Cue during a Continuous Performance Task

    Get PDF
    The allocation of attention modulates negative emotional processing in the amygdala. However, the role of passive exposure time to emotional signals in the modulation of amygdala activity during active task performance has not been examined. In two functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) experiments conducted in two different groups of healthy human subjects, we examined activation in the amygdala due to cued anticipation of painful stimuli while subjects performed a simple continuous performance task (CPT) with either a fixed or a parametrically varied trial duration. In the first experiment (N = 16), engagement in the CPT during a task with fixed trial duration produced the expected attenuation of amygdala activation, but close analysis suggested that the attenuation occurred during the period of active engagement in CPT, and that amygdala activity increased proportionately during the remainder of each trial, when subjects were passively exposed to the pain cue. In the second experiment (N = 12), the duration of each trial was parametrically varied, and we found that amygdala activation was linearly related to the time of passive exposure to the anticipatory cue. We suggest that amygdala activation during negative anticipatory processing depends directly on the passive exposure time to the negative cue

    An fMRI Study to Analyze Neural Correlates of Presence during Virtual Reality Experiences

    Full text link
    [EN] In the field of virtual reality (VR), many efforts have been made to analyze presence, the sense of being in the virtual world. However, it is only recently that functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been used to study presence during an automatic navigation through a virtual environment. In the present work, our aim was to use fMRI to study the sense of presence during a VR-free navigation task, in comparison with visualization of photographs and videos (automatic navigations through the same environment). The main goal was to analyze the usefulness of fMRI for this purpose, evaluating whether, in this context, the interaction between the subject and the environment is performed naturally, hiding the role of technology in the experience. We monitored 14 right-handed healthy females aged between 19 and 25 years. Frontal, parietal and occipital regions showed their involvement during free virtual navigation. Moreover, activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was also shown to be negatively correlated to sense of presence and the postcentral parietal cortex and insula showed a parametric increased activation according to the condition-related sense of presence, which suggests that stimulus attention and self-awareness processes related to the insula may be linked to the sense of presence.This study was funded by the Ministerio de EducaciĂłn y Ciencia Spain, Project Game Teen (TIN2010-20187) and partially by projects Consolider-C (SEJ2006-14301/PSIC), ‘CIBER of Physiopathology of Obesity and Nutrition, an initiative of ISCIII’, the Excellence Research Program PROMETEO (Generalitat Valenciana. Conselleria de EducaciĂłn, 2008-157) and the Consolider INGENIO program (CSD2007-00012). The work of Miriam Clemente was supported by the Generalitat Valenciana under a VALi+d Grant.Clemente Bellido, M.; Rey, B.; RodrĂ­guez Pujadas, A.; Barros Loscertales, A.; Baños, RM.; Botella, C.; Alcañiz Raya, ML.... (2014). An fMRI Study to Analyze Neural Correlates of Presence during Virtual Reality Experiences. Interacting with Computers. 26(3):269-284. https://doi.org/10.1093/iwc/iwt037S269284263Aguirre, G. K., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., & D’Esposito, M. (1996). The Parahippocampus Subserves Topographical Learning in Man. Cerebral Cortex, 6(6), 823-829. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.6.823Alcañiz, M., Rey, B., Tembl, J., & Parkhutik, V. (2009). A Neuroscience Approach to Virtual Reality Experience Using Transcranial Doppler Monitoring. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 18(2), 97-111. doi:10.1162/pres.18.2.97Amaro, E., & Barker, G. J. (2006). Study design in fMRI: Basic principles. Brain and Cognition, 60(3), 220-232. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2005.11.009Astur, R. S., St. Germain, S. A., Baker, E. K., Calhoun, V., Pearlson, G. D., & Constable, R. T. (2005). fMRI Hippocampal Activity During a VirtualRadial Arm Maze. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 30(3), 307-317. doi:10.1007/s10484-005-6385-zBaños, R. M., Botella, C., Garcia-Palacios, A., Villa, H., Perpiña, C., & Alcañiz, M. (2000). Presence and Reality Judgment in Virtual Environments: A Unitary Construct? CyberPsychology & Behavior, 3(3), 327-335. doi:10.1089/10949310050078760Baumann, S., Neff, C., Fetzick, S., Stangl, G., Basler, L., Vereneck, R., & Schneider, W. (2003). A Virtual Reality System for Neurobehavioral and Functional MRI Studies. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 6(3), 259-266. doi:10.1089/109493103322011542Maertens, M. (2008). Retinotopic activation in response to subjective contours in primary visual cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 2, 1-7. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.002.2008Baumgartner, T., Valko, L., Esslen, M., & JĂ€ncke, L. (2006). Neural Correlate of Spatial Presence in an Arousing and Noninteractive Virtual Reality: An EEG and Psychophysiology Study. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9(1), 30-45. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9.30Belliveau, J., Kennedy, D., McKinstry, R., Buchbinder, B., Weisskoff, R., Cohen, M., 
 Rosen, B. (1991). Functional mapping of the human visual cortex by magnetic resonance imaging. Science, 254(5032), 716-719. doi:10.1126/science.1948051Born, R. T., & Bradley, D. C. (2005). STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION OF VISUAL AREA MT. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28(1), 157-189. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131052Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115(1), 33-42. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.115.1.33Clemente, M., RodrĂ­guez, A., Rey, B., RodrĂ­guez, A., Baños, R. M., Botella, C., 
 Ávila, C. (2011). Analyzing the Level of Presence While Navigating in a Virtual Environment during an fMRI Scan. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 475-478. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-23768-3_61(Bud) Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel — now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 59-70. doi:10.1038/nrn2555Dilger, S., Straube, T., Mentzel, H.-J., Fitzek, C., Reichenbach, J. R., Hecht, H., 
 Miltner, W. H. R. (2003). Brain activation to phobia-related pictures in spider phobic humans: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 348(1), 29-32. doi:10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00647-5Dodds, C. M., Morein-Zamir, S., & Robbins, T. W. (2010). Dissociating Inhibition, Attention, and Response Control in the Frontoparietal Network Using Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Cerebral Cortex, 21(5), 1155-1165. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq187Epstein, R., & Kanwisher, N. (1998). A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature, 392(6676), 598-601. doi:10.1038/33402Flach, J. M., & Holden, J. G. (1998). The Reality of Experience: Gibson’s Way. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7(1), 90-95. doi:10.1162/105474698565550Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Poline, J.-B., Grasby, P. J., Williams, S. C. R., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Turner, R. (1995). Analysis of fMRI Time-Series Revisited. NeuroImage, 2(1), 45-53. doi:10.1006/nimg.1995.1007GEAKE, J., & HANSEN, P. (2005). Neural correlates of intelligence as revealed by fMRI of fluid analogies. NeuroImage, 26(2), 555-564. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.035Haldane, M., Cunningham, G., Androutsos, C., & Frangou, S. (2008). Structural brain correlates of response inhibition in Bipolar Disorder I. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22(2), 138-143. doi:10.1177/0269881107082955Hartley, T., Maguire, E. A., Spiers, H. J., & Burgess, N. (2003). The Well-Worn Route and the Path Less Traveled. Neuron, 37(5), 877-888. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00095-3Heeter, C. (1992). Being There: The Subjective Experience of Presence. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 1(2), 262-271. doi:10.1162/pres.1992.1.2.262De Castro, F. (2009). Wiring olfaction: the cellular and molecular mechanisms that guide the development of synaptic connections from the nose to the cortex. Frontiers in Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/neuro.22.004.2009Johnson, P. B., Ferraina, S., Bianchi, L., & Caminiti, R. (1996). Cortical Networks for Visual Reaching: Physiological and Anatomical Organization of Frontal and Parietal Lobe Arm Regions. Cerebral Cortex, 6(2), 102-119. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.2.102Karnath, H.-O. (2005). Awareness of the Functioning of One’s Own Limbs Mediated by the Insular Cortex? Journal of Neuroscience, 25(31), 7134-7138. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1590-05.2005Koechlin, E. (2003). The Architecture of Cognitive Control in the Human Prefrontal Cortex. Science, 302(5648), 1181-1185. doi:10.1126/science.1088545Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., Fitzsimmons, J. R., Cuthbert, B. N., Scott, J. D., Moulder, B., & Nangia, V. (1998). Emotional arousal and activation of the visual cortex: An fMRI analysis. Psychophysiology, 35(2), 199-210. doi:10.1017/s0048577298001991Le Bihan, D., Turner, R., Zeffiro, T. A., Cuenod, C. A., Jezzard, P., & Bonnerot, V. (1993). Activation of human primary visual cortex during visual recall: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 90(24), 11802-11805. doi:10.1073/pnas.90.24.11802Lessiter, J., Freeman, J., Keogh, E., & Davidoff, J. (2001). A Cross-Media Presence Questionnaire: The ITC-Sense of Presence Inventory. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 10(3), 282-297. doi:10.1162/105474601300343612Loomis, J. M. (1992). Distal Attribution and Presence. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 1(1), 113-119. doi:10.1162/pres.1992.1.1.113Mellet, E., Laou, L., Petit, L., Zago, L., Mazoyer, B., & Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2009). Impact of the virtual reality on the neural representation of an environment. Human Brain Mapping, 31(7), 1065-1075. doi:10.1002/hbm.20917Mishkin, M., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1982). Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behavioural Brain Research, 6(1), 57-77. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(82)90081-xMraz, R., Hong, J., Quintin, G., Staines, W. R., McIlroy, W. E., Zakzanis, K. K., & Graham, S. J. (2003). A Platform for Combining Virtual Reality Experiments with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 6(4), 359-368. doi:10.1089/109493103322278736Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking Feelings: An fMRI Study of the Cognitive Regulation of Emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215-1229. doi:10.1162/089892902760807212Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97-113. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4Owen, A. M., Downes, J. J., Sahakian, B. J., Polkey, C. E., & Robbins, T. W. (1990). Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 28(10), 1021-1034. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(90)90137-dPerani, D., Fazio, F., Borghese, N. A., Tettamanti, M., Ferrari, S., Decety, J., & Gilardi, M. C. (2001). Different Brain Correlates for Watching Real and Virtual Hand Actions. NeuroImage, 14(3), 749-758. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0872Petrides, M. (2000). The role of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in working memory. Experimental Brain Research, 133(1), 44-54. doi:10.1007/s002210000399Pine, D. S., Grun, J., Maguire, E. A., Burgess, N., Zarahn, E., Koda, V., 
 Bilder, R. M. (2002). Neurodevelopmental Aspects of Spatial Navigation: A Virtual Reality fMRI Study. NeuroImage, 15(2), 396-406. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0988Riva, G., Waterworth, J. A., Waterworth, E. L., & Mantovani, F. (2011). From intention to action: The role of presence. New Ideas in Psychology, 29(1), 24-37. doi:10.1016/j.newideapsych.2009.11.002Rey, B., Alcañiz, M., Tembl, J., & Parkhutik, V. (2009). Brain activity and presence: a preliminary study in different immersive conditions using transcranial Doppler monitoring. Virtual Reality, 14(1), 55-65. doi:10.1007/s10055-009-0141-2Sanchez-Vives, M. V., & Slater, M. (2005). From presence to consciousness through virtual reality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(4), 332-339. doi:10.1038/nrn1651Scheibe, C., Wartenburger, I., WĂŒstenberg, T., Kathmann, N., Villringer, A., & Heekeren, H. R. (2006). Neural correlates of the interaction between transient and sustained processes: A mixed blocked/event-related fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 27(7), 545-551. doi:10.1002/hbm.20199Schuemie, M. J., van der Straaten, P., Krijn, M., & van der Mast, C. A. P. G. (2001). Research on Presence in Virtual Reality: A Survey. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 4(2), 183-201. doi:10.1089/109493101300117884Smith, S. M. (2004). Overview of fMRI analysis. The British Journal of Radiology, 77(suppl_2), S167-S175. doi:10.1259/bjr/33553595Usoh, M., Catena, E., Arman, S., & Slater, M. (2000). Using Presence Questionnaires in Reality. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 9(5), 497-503. doi:10.1162/105474600566989Vanni, S., Tanskanen, T., Seppa, M., Uutela, K., & Hari, R. (2001). Coinciding early activation of the human primary visual cortex and anteromedial cuneus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(5), 2776-2780. doi:10.1073/pnas.041600898Wolf, U., Rapoport, M. J., & Schweizer, T. A. (2009). Evaluating the Affective Component of the Cerebellar Cognitive Affective Syndrome. Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 21(3), 245-253. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.21.3.245Zahorik, P., & Jenison, R. L. (1998). Presence as Being-in-the-World. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7(1), 78-89. doi:10.1162/10547469856554

    Ten years of Nature Reviews Neuroscience: insights from the highly cited

    Full text link

    Modular architectonic organization of the insula in the macaque monkey

    No full text
    In order to provide a framework for ongoing analyses of the neuronal connections of the insular cortex of the macaque monkey using modern high-resolution methods, we examined its anatomical organization in serial coronal sections stained alternately with Nissl and Gallyas (myelin) techniques. We observed the same 15 distinct architectonic areas in 10 brains. Within the granular, dysgranular, and agranular regions described in prior studies, we identified 4, 4, and 7 distinct areas, respectively. Across brains, these areas have consistent architectonic characteristics, and in flat map reconstructions they display a consistent topological or neighborhood arrangement, despite variations in the size of individual areas between cases. The borders between areas are generally rather sharply defined. Some areas, in particular the dysgranular areas, appear to consistently contain subtle transitions that suggest possible sub-areas or modules within the well-delimited areas. The presence of a distinct granular area that straddles the fundus of the superior limiting sulcus over its entire posterior-to-anterior extent is consistent with the available evidence on interoceptive thalamo-cortical projections, and also with the tensile anchor theory of species-specific cortical gyrification. These observations are consonant with the homeostatic afferent model of processing in the primate insula, and they suggest that discrete modules within insular cortex provide the basis for its polymodal integration of all salient activity relevant to ongoing emotional behavior
    • 

    corecore