1,302 research outputs found

    A COMPARISON OF UNDERWATER GLIDING AND KICKING TECHNIQUES

    Get PDF
    Net forces created when towing swimmers through water were examined for gliding and undelwater kicking. Sixteen experienced male swimmers of similar body shape were towed through water via a motorised winch and pulley system. A load cell measured net force (propulsive force - drag force) at velocities of 1.6, 1.9, 2.2, 2.5 and 3.1 ms-', respectively. At each velocity swimmers performed a prone streamline glide; lateral streamline glide; prone freestyle kick; prone dolphin kick; and lateral dolphin kick. A 2- way repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant differences between the gliding and kicking conditions at different velocities. Results suggest that there is an optimal velocity at which to begin undelwater kicking in order to prevent energy loss from excessive active drag

    POSTURAL RESPONSE TO VARIED VELOCITY AND DURATION VISUAL PERTURBATIONS IN VIRTUAL REALITY

    Get PDF
    The purpose of this study was to examine the center of pressure response to visual perturbations presented in virtual reality (VR). The visual perturbations were an anterior or posterior displacement that varied in velocity (1m/s, 3m/s & 5m/s) and duration (0.6s, 1s and 2 s). The center of pressure distance travelled was measured in the anterior-posterior direction while subjects balance on one foot. Four subjects were evaluated in the pilot. A t-test was used to examine the pooled effect of the perturbations. The visual perturbation significantly increased the distance the center of pressure travelled (p = 0.041). A 2-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to assess the specific affects velocity and duration. The main effect for duration was significant (p =0.014). Duration of the visual perturbation appears to be the most important component to challenge postural control

    MOTOR CONTROL PATTERNS IN ELITE SWIMMERS’ FREESTYLE STROKE DURING DRYLAND SWIMMING

    Get PDF
    The purpose of this study was to compare motor control patterns of elite freestyle swimmers when asked to swim at 100m freestyle pace using a dryland swimbench. Collegiate and masters level swimmers (n=15) whose 100m freestyle time were faster than 75% of the FINA cutoff time, performed four 10 second trials of freestyle swimming on a dryland swimbench. 3-D kinematic analysis was used to calculate displacement in the hand in the cranial-caudal, vertical, and medial-lateral directions. A 2-way repeated measures ANOVA was used to compare hand path between swimmers and within trials (n=58). Data was not statically significant, but three distinct combinations of hand paths were used to perform the 100m freestyle task on the swimbench. These hand paths differed from historical in-water data. Findings imply individual swimmers adjusted kinematics on the swimbench to accommodate for environmental constraints

    āļāļēāļĢāļžāļąāļ’āļ™āļēāļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļŠāļģāļŦāļĢāļąāļšāļāļķāļāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļ—āļĩāđˆāļŠāļēāļĄāļēāļĢāļ–āļ›āļĢāļąāļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļāļ§āđ‰āļēāļ‡āđ„āļ”āđ‰

    Get PDF
    āļŦāļĨāļąāļāļāļēāļĢāđāļĨāļ°āđ€āļŦāļ•āļļāļœāļĨ: āļāļēāļĢāļāļķāļāđƒāļŦāđ‰āļœāļđāđ‰āļ›āđˆāļ§āļĒāļ—āļēāļ‡āļĢāļ°āļšāļšāļ›āļĢāļ°āļŠāļēāļ—āđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āđ‚āļ”āļĒāļĄāļĩāļāļēāļĢāļŠāđˆāļ§āļĒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ‚āļ­āļ‡āļœāļđāđ‰āļ›āđˆāļ§āļĒāđƒāļ™āļ‚āļ“āļ°āđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļšāļ™āļĨāļđāđˆāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļ—āļģāđƒāļŦāđ‰āļāļēāļĢāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļ‚āļ­āļ‡āļœāļđāđ‰āļ›āđˆāļ§āļĒāļ”āļĩāļ‚āļķāđ‰āļ™ āđāļ•āđˆāļāļēāļĢāđƒāļŠāđ‰āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļŠāļģāļŦāļĢāļąāļšāļāļķāļāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āđƒāļ™āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĻāđ„āļ—āļĒāļĒāļąāļ‡āđ„āļĄāđˆāđ€āļ›āđ‡āļ™āļ—āļĩāđˆāđāļžāļĢāđˆāļŦāļĨāļēāļĒ āđ€āļ™āļ·āđˆāļ­āļ‡āļˆāļēāļāļĄāļĩāļĢāļēāļ„āļēāđāļžāļ‡āđāļĨāļ°āļ•āđ‰āļ­āļ‡āļ™āļģāđ€āļ‚āđ‰āļēāļˆāļēāļāļ•āđˆāļēāļ‡āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĻ āļ­āļĩāļāļ—āļąāđ‰āļ‡āļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļāļ§āđ‰āļēāļ‡āļ‚āļ­āļ‡āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļāļģāļŦāļ™āļ”āđ„āļ§āđ‰āļ­āļĒāđˆāļēāļ‡āđāļ™āđˆāļ™āļ­āļ™ āļ—āļģāđƒāļŦāđ‰āđƒāļŠāđ‰āđ„āļ”āđ‰āļāļąāļšāļĨāļđāđˆāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āđ€āļžāļĩāļĒāļ‡āļšāļēāļ‡āļĢāļļāđˆāļ™āđ€āļ—āđˆāļēāļ™āļąāđ‰āļ™ āļ§āļąāļ•āļ–āļļāļ›āļĢāļ°āļŠāļ‡āļ„āđŒ: āđ€āļžāļ·āđˆāļ­āļ›āļĢāļ°āļ”āļīāļĐāļāđŒāļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļŠāļģāļŦāļĢāļąāļšāļāļķāļāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļ—āļĩāđˆāļ–āļ­āļ”āļ›āļĢāļ°āļāļ­āļšāđāļĨāļ°āļ›āļĢāļąāļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļāļ§āđ‰āļēāļ‡āđ„āļ”āđ‰ āļŠāļēāļĄāļēāļĢāļ–āļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāđ„āļ”āđ‰ 1 - 40 āļāļ. āļ§āļīāļ˜āļĩāļāļēāļĢāļĻāļķāļāļĐāļē: āļ›āļĢāļ°āļ”āļīāļĐāļāđŒāļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļŠāļēāļĄāļēāļĢāļ–āļ–āļ­āļ”āļ›āļĢāļ°āļāļ­āļšāđ„āļ”āđ‰ āļˆāļēāļāļ™āļąāđ‰āļ™āļ—āļ”āļŠāļ­āļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāđāļĄāđˆāļ™āļĒāļģāđƒāļ™āļāļēāļĢāļĒāļāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ‚āļ­āļ‡āđ€āļ„āļĢāļ·āđˆāļ­āļ‡ āđ‚āļ”āļĒāđ€āļ›āļĢāļĩāļĒāļšāđ€āļ—āļĩāļĒāļšāļāļąāļšāļ•āļļāđ‰āļĄāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļĄāļēāļ•āļĢāļāļēāļ™ āļ•āļąāđ‰āļ‡āđāļ•āđˆ 1 - 40 āļāļ. āđāļĨāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĩāļĒāļšāļāļąāļšāļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļ—āļĩāđˆāļ™āļģāđ€āļ‚āđ‰āļēāļˆāļēāļāļ•āđˆāļēāļ‡āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĻ (BiodexÂŪ) āđ‚āļ”āļĒāļĒāļāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ‚āļ“āļ°āļĒāļ·āļ™30% āļ‚āļ­āļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ•āļąāļ§ āđƒāļ™āļ„āļ™āļŠāļļāļ‚āļ āļēāļžāļ”āļĩ 15 āļ„āļ™ āļ—āļĩāđˆāļĄāļĩāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ­āļĒāļđāđˆāđƒāļ™āļŠāđˆāļ§āļ‡ 40 - 120 āļāļ. āđ‚āļ”āļĒāđ€āļ›āļĢāļĩāļĒāļšāđ€āļ—āļĩāļĒāļšāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļĒāļāđ„āļ”āđ‰āđāļĨāļ°āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāđ€āļŦāļĨāļ·āļ­āļšāļ™āđ€āļ„āļĢāļ·āđˆāļ­āļ‡āļŠāļąāđˆāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļ āļ§āļīāđ€āļ„āļĢāļēāļ°āļŦāđŒāļ‚āđ‰āļ­āļĄāļđāļĨāđ‚āļ”āļĒāđƒāļŠāđ‰āļŠāļ–āļīāļ•āļīāđāļšāļš intraclass correlation āđāļĨāļ°āļ—āļ”āļŠāļ­āļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļ„āļ‡āļ—āļĩāđˆāđƒāļ™āļāļēāļĢāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ‚āļ“āļ°āļāļķāļāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™ āđƒāļ™āļ„āļ™āļŠāļļāļ‚āļ āļēāļžāļ”āļĩāļˆāļģāļ™āļ§āļ™ 7 āļ„āļ™ āļ‚āļ“āļ°āđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļšāļ™āļĨāļđāđˆāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āđāļĨāļ°āđƒāļŠāđ‰āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļąāđ‰āļ‡ 2 āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ āļ— āđ‚āļ”āļĒāļšāļąāļ™āļ—āļķāļāļ„āđˆāļēāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļĒāļāļ‚āļķāđ‰āļ™āļ‚āļ“āļ°āđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļ—āļļāļ āđ† 5 āļ™āļēāļ—āļĩ āđāļĨāđ‰āļ§āļ™āļģāļĄāļēāļ—āļ”āļŠāļ­āļšāļ—āļēāļ‡āļŠāļ–āļīāļ•āļīāļ”āđ‰āļ§āļĒ 2-way repeated measures ANOVA āļœāļĨāļāļēāļĢāļĻāļķāļāļĐāļē: āļœāļĨāļāļēāļĢāļ—āļ”āļŠāļ­āļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāđāļĄāđˆāļ™āļĒāļģāđƒāļ™āļāļēāļĢāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļāļąāļšāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļĄāļēāļ•āļĢāļāļēāļ™ āļžāļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļ„āļĨāļēāļ”āđ€āļ„āļĨāļ·āđˆāļ­āļ™āļˆāļēāļāļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļĄāļēāļ•āļĢāļāļēāļ™āļ„āļīāļ”āđ€āļ›āđ‡āļ™ 2% āļ‚āļ­āļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļĄāļēāļ•āļĢāļāļēāļ™āļœāļĨāļāļēāļĢāļ—āļ”āļŠāļ­āļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāđāļĄāđˆāļ™āļĒāļģāđƒāļ™āļāļēāļĢāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāđ‚āļ”āļĒāđ€āļ—āļĩāļĒāļšāļāļąāļšāļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļ—āļĩāđˆāļ™āļģāđ€āļ‚āđ‰āļēāļˆāļēāļāļ•āđˆāļēāļ‡āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĻ āļžāļšāļ§āđˆāļēāļĄāļĩāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļŠāļąāļĄāļžāļąāļ™āļ˜āđŒāđ„āļ›āđƒāļ™āļ—āļīāļĻāļ—āļēāļ‡āđ€āļ”āļĩāļĒāļ§āļāļąāļ™āļ­āļĒāđˆāļēāļ‡āļĄāļĩāļ™āļąāļĒāļŠāļģāļ„āļąāļāļ—āļēāļ‡āļŠāļ–āļīāļ•āļī(r = 0.993) āđāļĨāļ°āļœāļĨāļāļēāļĢāļ—āļ”āļŠāļ­āļšāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļ„āļ‡āļ—āļĩāđˆāđƒāļ™āļāļēāļĢāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ‚āļ­āļ‡āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ‚āļ“āļ°āđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āđƒāļ™āđāļ•āđˆāļĨāļ°āļŠāđˆāļ§āļ‡āđ€āļ§āļĨāļē āļĢāļ°āļŦāļ§āđˆāļēāļ‡āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļ™āļģāđ€āļ‚āđ‰āļēāļˆāļēāļāļ•āđˆāļēāļ‡āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĻ āđāļĨāļ°āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļ›āļĢāļ°āļ”āļīāļĐāļāđŒāļ‚āļķāđ‰āļ™ āļžāļšāļ§āđˆāļēāļ—āļąāđ‰āļ‡ 2 āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļĄāļĩāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāļ„āļ‡āļ—āļĩāđˆāđƒāļ™āļāļēāļĢāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ•āļĨāļ­āļ”āļŠāđˆāļ§āļ‡āđ€āļ§āļĨāļē 20 āļ™āļēāļ—āļĩ āļ‚āļ“āļ°āđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļšāļ™āļĨāļđāđˆāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āđ€āļ—āļĩāļĒāļšāđ€āļ—āđˆāļēāļāļąāļ™āļŠāļĢāļļāļ›: āļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļ›āļĢāļ°āļ”āļīāļĐāļāđŒāļ‚āļķāđ‰āļ™āđƒāļŦāļĄāđˆāļ™āļąāđ‰āļ™ āļĄāļĩāđ‚āļ„āļĢāļ‡āļŠāļĢāđ‰āļēāļ‡āļ—āļĩāđˆāļŠāļēāļĄāļēāļĢāļ–āļ–āļ­āļ”āļ›āļĢāļ°āļāļ­āļšāđ„āļ”āđ‰āļ‡āđˆāļēāļĒ āļāļēāļĢāļ‚āļ™āļĒāđ‰āļēāļĒāļŠāļ°āļ”āļ§āļ āļŠāļēāļĄāļēāļĢāļ–āļ›āļĢāļ°āļāļ­āļšāđ€āļ‚āđ‰āļēāļāļąāļšāļĨāļđāđˆāđ€āļ”āļīāļ™āļŠāđˆāļ§āļ™āđƒāļŦāļāđˆāļ—āļĩāđˆāđƒāļŠāđ‰āļ āļēāļĒāđƒāļ™āļĻāļđāļ™āļĒāđŒāļŸāļ·āđ‰āļ™āļŸāļđāļ āļēāļĒāđƒāļ™āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĻāđ„āļ—āļĒ āđāļĨāļ°āļĄāļĩāļ„āļ§āļēāļĄāđāļĄāđˆāļ™āļĒāļģāđƒāļ™āļāļēāļĢāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāđ€āļ—āļĩāļĒāļšāđ€āļ—āđˆāļēāļāļąāļšāļ­āļļāļ›āļāļĢāļ“āđŒāļžāļĒāļļāļ‡āļ™āđ‰āļģāļŦāļ™āļąāļāļ—āļĩāđˆāļ™āļģāđ€āļ‚āđ‰āļēāļˆāļēāļāļ•āđˆāļēāļ‡āļ›āļĢāļ°āđ€āļ—āļĻāļ„āļģāļŠāļģāļ„āļąāļ: body weight support, adjustable BWS, gait rehabilitation, treadmill, gait training_

    Positional influence of center of masticatory forces on occlusal contact forces using a digital occlusal analyzer

    Get PDF
    Statement of problem. Digital occlusal analyzers allow the recording of dental contact forces. Some authors assume a unique location for the center of contact forces at the position of maximum intercuspation, while others indicate variations in dental contact forces when recorded at different times of the day. Which approach is more appropriate is unclear. Purpose. The purpose of this in vitro study was to analyze whether a change in the balance of masticatory forces influences the location of the center of contact forces and its magnitude. Material and methods. Three different dental casts, selected under dental criteria, were mounted in maximum intercuspation on a semiadjustable articulator equipped with a pattern indicating 9 different force application points (intersection point between 3 longitudinal rows and 3 transverse columns). A force of constant magnitude (169 N) was applied 10 times at each of the application points, and occlusal forces were recorded with a digital occlusal analyzer. Then, two variables were studied: the location of the center of contact forces and its magnitude. Each force application position (9 positions × 3 dental casts=27 in total) was repeated 10 times, and measured data were statistically analyzed with 2-way repeated measures ANOVA (a=.05) test. Results. The repeatability of the method indicated that the coefficient of variation mean was 0.37% in the location of the center of contact forces and that its magnitude was 3.70%. The 2-way repeated measures ANOVA test revealed statistically significant variations in the location of the center of contact forces and its magnitude, revealing that longitudinal changes of the application point of masticatory forces affected the magnitude of contact forces and that longitudinal and transverse changes of the application point of masticatory forces affected the location of the center of contact forces. Conclusions. The location of the center of contact force and its magnitude provided by a digital occlusal analyzer at the position of maximum intercuspation are not necessarily unique to each articulated dental cast. Even if the intensity of the masticatory force remains unchanged, changes in its lateral or longitudinal balance also influence the result of the occlusion forces. (J Prosthet Dent 2023;129:930.e1–e8he authors thank the University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU) for providing the DEHI research laboratory (www.ehu.eus/dehi). The authors also thank Ayesta dental practice and Odontolan dental laboratory for their assistance

    Half-time and high-speed running in the second half of soccer

    Get PDF
    This study investigated if the quantity of high-speed running (movements >15 km.h-1) completed in the first 15 minutes of competitive football matches differed from that completed in the corresponding 15 minutes of the second half. Twenty semi-professional soccer players (age 21.2 Âą 3.6 years, body mass 76.4 Âą 3.8 kg, height 1.89 Âą 0.05 m) participated in the study. Fifty competitive soccer matches and 192 data files were analysed (4 Âą 2 files per match) using Global Positioning Satellite technology. Data were analysed using 2-way repeated measures ANOVA and Pearson correlations. No differences were found between the first 15 min of each half for the distance completed at high-speed (>15 km.h-1) or sprinting (>21 km.h-1), or in the number of sprints undertaken (p>0.05). However, total distance covered was shorter (1st half vs. 2nd half: 1746 Âą 220 vs. 1644 Âą 224 m; p<0.001) and mean speed lower (1st half vs. 2nd half: 7.0 Âą 0.9 vs. 6.6 Âą 0.9 km.h-1; p<0.001) in the first 15 min of the second half compared to the first. The correlations between the duration of the half-time interval and the difference in the high-speed running or sprinting between first and second halves (0-15 min) were very small (r=0.08 [p=0.25] and r=0.04 [p=0.61] respectively). Therefore, this study did not find any difference between the amount of high-speed running and sprinting completed by semi-professional soccer players when the first 15 minutes of the first and second half of competitive matches were compared The maintenance of high-speed running and sprinting, as total distance and mean speed declined, may be a function of the pacing strategies adopted by players in competitive matches

    Carbohydrate Mouth Rinse Improves 1.5 h Run Performance: Is There a Dose-Effect?

    Get PDF
    There is a substantial body of recent evidence showing ergogenic effects of carbohydrate (CHO) mouth rinsing on endurance performance. However, there is a lack of research on the dose-effect and the aim of this study was to investigate the effect of two different concentrations (6% and 12% weight/volume, w/v) on 90 minute treadmill running performance. Seven active males took part in one familiarization trial and three experimental trials (90-minute self-paced performance trials). Solutions (placebo, 6% or 12% CHO-electrolyte solution, CHO-E) were rinsed in the mouth at the beginning, and at 15, 30 and 45 minutes during the run. The total distance covered was greater during the CHO-E trials (6%, 14.6 Âą 1.7 km; 12%, 14.9 Âą 1.6 km) compared to the placebo trial (13.9 Âą 1.7 km, P 0.05). There were no between trial differences (P > 0.05) in ratings of perceived exertion (RPE) and feeling or arousal ratings suggesting that the same subjective ratings were associated with higher speeds in the CHO-E trials. Enhanced performance in the CHO-E trials was due to higher speeds in the last 30 minutes even though rinses were not provided during the final 45 minutes, suggesting the effects persist for at least 20-45 minutes after rinsing. In conclusion, mouth rinsing with a CHO-E solution enhanced endurance running performance but there does not appear to be a dose-response effect with the higher concentration (12%) compared to a standard 6% solution

    A Subgroup Analysis of the Impact of Self-testing Frequency on Examination Scores in a Pathophysiology Course

    Get PDF
    Objective: To determine if the frequency of self-testing of course material prior to actual examination improves examination scores, regardless of the actual scores on the self-testing. Methods: Practice quizzes were randomly generated from a total of 1342 multiple-choice questions in pathophysiology and made available online for student self-testing. Intercorrelations, 2-way repeated measures ANOVA with post hoc tests, and 2-group comparisons following rank ordering, were conducted. Results: During each of 4 testing blocks, more than 85% of students took advantage of the self-testing process for a total of 7042 attempts. A consistent significant correlation (pâ‰Ī0.05) existed between the number of practice quiz attempts and the subsequent examination scores. No difference in the number of quiz attempts was demonstrated compared to the first testing block. Exam scores for the first and second testing blocks were both higher than those for third and fourth blocks. Conclusion: Although self-testing strategies increase retrieval and retention, they are uncommon in pharmacy education. The results suggested that the number of self-testing attempts alone improved subsequent examination scores, regardless of the score for self-tests

    Gait analysis in a <i>Mecp2</i> knockout mouse model of Rett syndrome reveals early-onset and progressive motor deficits

    Get PDF
    Rett syndrome (RTT) is a genetic disorder characterized by a range of features including cognitive impairment, gait abnormalities and a reduction in purposeful hand skills. Mice harbouring knockout mutations in the &lt;i&gt;Mecp2&lt;/i&gt; gene display many RTT-like characteristics and are central to efforts to find novel therapies for the disorder. As hand stereotypies and gait abnormalities constitute major diagnostic criteria in RTT, it is clear that motor and gait-related phenotypes will be of importance in assessing preclinical therapeutic outcomes. We therefore aimed to assess gait properties over the prodromal phase in a functional knockout mouse model of RTT. In male &lt;i&gt;Mecp2&lt;/i&gt; knockout mice, we observed alterations in stride, coordination and balance parameters at 4 weeks of age, before the onset of other overt phenotypic changes as revealed by observational scoring. These data suggest that gait measures may be used as a robust and early marker of &lt;i&gt;Mecp2&lt;/i&gt;-dysfunction in future preclinical therapeutic studies
    • â€Ķ
    corecore