584 research outputs found

    Nutrition in the Nursery

    Get PDF
    Miriam Lowenberg, M.S. ,29, helps wartime nurseries solve food problems, says Phyllis Le

    Are octopuses special? Mind, sociality and life history

    Get PDF
    Understanding the Umwelt or being-ness of an octopus is a fascinating problem. Mather’s review provides us with significant insights into the ways of living of non-humans that exploit a perceptual and physical world we can only guess at. Octopus “distributed minds” call into question our primate-based understandings of the importance of sociality and the pace of life in the evolution of complex perceptual and behavioural abilities

    Are octopuses special? Mind, sociality and life history

    Get PDF
    Understanding the Umwelt or being-ness of an octopus is a fascinating problem. Mather’s review provides us with significant insights into the ways of living of non-humans that exploit a perceptual and physical world we can only guess at. Octopus “distributed minds” call into question our primate-based understandings of the importance of sociality and the pace of life in the evolution of complex perceptual and behavioural abilities

    A demographic profile and attitudinal study of donors to the Tennessee 4-H program

    Get PDF
    The purpose of the study was twofold. The first purpose was to develop a demographic profile of the individual and corporate donors to the Tennessee 4-H program. The second purpose was to measure these donors\u27 attitudes toward the Tennessee 4-H program and determine if attitude in any way affected the donors contributions to the Tennessee 4-H program. The final sample was composed of 567 randomly selected individual and corporate donors. The donors returned a mailed questionnaire developed to measure donors\u27 total opinion of the Tennessee 4-H program, perceptions about the 4-H objectives, perceptions about the life skills 4-H attempts to teach youth, and gather demographic information. The individual donors were described according to gender, age, residence, education, involvement with 4-H, employment status, income, and philanthropic history. Corporate donors were described according to gender, age, involvement with 4-H, employment status, and philanthropic history. Additionally, the demographic variables were described along with their relationship to the donors\u27 philanthropic giving patterns. The attitude measurements were described along with their relationship to the demographic variables and were correlated with philanthropic giving patterns. The study revealed that the majority of donors, both individual and corporate, were male. Most were aged 30 to 44 or 45 to 59 and had B.S. degrees or advanced degrees. Descriptive statistics and appropriate inferential tests were used in the analysis. The demographic variables were compared with the philanthropic giving pattern to determine if any relationships existed. According to the findings, the demographic variable education had a relationship to the amount of money contributed in 1990-91 for individual donors and corporate donors. Those donors with bachelor\u27s degrees and those with advanced degrees indicated donating more money to 4-H in 1990-91 than those with a high school education. From the donors responding, the majority of the individual donors indicated they would be receptive to a more aggressive fundraising campaign for the Tennessee 4-H program. However, of the corporate donors responding, the majority would not be receptive to a more aggressive fund raising campaign. Finally, there was no correlation found between donors\u27 attitudes about 4-H and their philanthropic giving patterns

    Global ivory trade has been banned for decades, so why are there still stockpiles?

    Get PDF
    First paragraph: China is the most recent nation to destroy its ivory stockpile. It is the world’s largest market for illegal ivory, and the move is welcome news for threatened elephant populations. Read article on The Conversation website: https://theconversation.com/global-ivory-trade-has-been-banned-for-decades-so-why-are-there-still-stockpiles-2202

    Can We Sustainably Harvest Ivory?

    Get PDF
    This work was unfunded, DL and PL thank their families for their comprehension. PL thanks C. Moss and many private donors for supporting the Amboseli data collection. We thank Enrico Pirotta for early discussions about the model and reviewers whose suggestions improved the manuscript.Peer reviewedPostprintPostprin

    Status of Zanzibar red colobus and Sykes's monkeys in two coastal forests in 2005

    Get PDF
    We censused two discrete subpopulations of the endangered Zanzibar red colobus (Procolobus kirkii) and sympatric Sykes's monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis albogularis) between February 2004 and September 2005 in two coastal forests in Zanzibar, including the northernmost (Kiwengwa-Pongwe Forest) and some of the southernmost (Uzi and Vundwe Islands) extents of P. kirkii's range. Surveys totalled 365 hours and 307.8 km along >16 km of line transects; 472 sightings were made (Red colobus n = 252; Sykes's monkey n = 220). The southern forests (coral rag with adjacent mangrove) of Uzi and Vundwe Islands were found to support P. kirkii at a higher estimated density (29.6 groups/km²) relative to the northern coral rag forest (lacking mangrove) in Kiwengwa-Pongwe (7.5 groups/km²). However, for red colobus in coral rag, up to c.40% these "groups" could be "sub-groups" based on our observations of consistent fission-fusion and small groups seen during the census (6.63 ± 0.4SE in Kiwengwa, 6.64 ± 0.33SE in Uzi) and behavioral follows in this habitat type. We therefore also include density estimates expressed as individuals/km² (49.72 individuals/km2 in Kiwengwa, 196.32 individuals/km² in Uzi). On Uzi and Vundwe Islands, colobus densities were higher than those of Sykes's monkey (Sykes's monkey density = 18.9 groups/km²), while we found no difference between the density of these two taxa in Kiwengwa (Sykes's monkey density = 8.1 groups/km²) where encounters with humans were more frequent, vegetation was more disturbed, and the two species often associated. Although these populations represented a fraction of the historical total population of red colobus and Sykes's monkeys on Zanzibar, their estimated abundance was significant, and their marginal habitat and unprotected status were important in the general context of primate conservation in unprotected and fragmented landscapes

    Wild Western Lowland gorillas signal selectively using odor

    Get PDF
    Mammals communicate socially through visual, auditory and chemical signals. The chemical sense is the oldest sense and is shared by all organisms including bacteria. Despite mounting evidence for social chemo-signalling in humans, the extent to which it modulates behavior is debated and can benefit from comparative models of closely related hominoids. The use of odor cues in wild ape social communication has been only rarely explored. Apart from one study on wild chimpanzee sniffing, our understanding is limited to anecdotes. We present the first study of wild gorilla chemo-communication and the first analysis of olfactory signalling in relation to arousal levels and odor strength in wild apes. If gorilla scent is used as a signalling mechanism instead of only a sign of arousal or stress, odor emission should be context specific and capable of variation as a function of the relationships between the emitter and perceiver(s). Measured through a human pungency scale, we determined the factors that predicted extreme levels of silverback odor for one wild western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) group silverback. Extreme silverback odor was predicted by the presence and intensity of inter-unit interactions, silverback anger, distress and long-calling auditory rates, and the absence of close proximity between the silverback and mother of the youngest infant. Odor strength also varied according to the focal silverback's strategic responses during high intensity inter-unit interactions. Silverbacks appear to use odor as a modifiable form of communication; where odor acts as a highly flexible, context dependent signalling mechanism to group members and extra-group units. The importance of olfaction to ape social communication may be especially pertinent in Central African forests where limited visibility may necessitate increased reliance on other senses

    Play in Elephants: Wellbeing, Welfare or Distraction?

    Get PDF
    We explore elephant play behaviour since (a) play has been proposed to represent a potential welfare indicator; and (b) play has been associated with long-term survival in the wild. We categorised play into four types, and investigate both social (gentle, escalated-contact) and non-social (lone-locomotor, exploratory-object) play from observations made on wild (Asian N = 101; African N = 130) and captive (Asian N = 8; African N = 7) elephant calves ranging in age from birth to five years. Social play was the most frequent type of play among immature elephants, accounting for an average of 3%–9% of active time. Non-social play accounted for an additional 1%–11% of time. The most time spent in play was seen in captive Asian calves, particularly at the ages of 1–6 months, while wild African calves spent the least time in play overall, even though they had the greatest number and most diverse range of play partners available. We assessed calf energetics using time spent suckling, resting, moving and independent feeding. Time spent playing was unrelated to time spent suckling but negatively associated with time spent independently feeding. There were no associations with time spent moving or resting. Maternal energy via lactation was unrelated to play early in life, but energy acquired independently may constrain or enable play. Play, while a potential indicator of compromised welfare for many species when absent, can act as a highly stimulating activity for captive elephants in the absence of other forms of arousal

    A “halfway house” for improving captive welfare

    Get PDF
    It is certainly time to aim for higher quality management strategies for Thailand’s captive elephants, and to engage with sustainable livelihoods for traditional mahouts. Baker & Winkler’s proposal to rewild Thai elephants by placing them under the guardianship of Karen mahouts is recognized as not “wild” since it remains a form of management requiring elephants to live under the control of human caregivers. We applaud the positive welfare aims of this proposal; however, we caution that few of the long-term consequences for elephants or habitats can be known, and further considerations of elephant population dynamics and forest ecosystems are required if these proposals are to be successful for conservation and elephant welfare
    • 

    corecore