611 research outputs found

    Reflectance, illumination, and appearance in color constancy

    Get PDF
    We studied color constancy using a pair of identical 3-D Color Mondrian displays. We viewed one 3-D Mondrian in nearly uniform illumination, and the other in directional, nonuniform illumination. We used the three dimensional structures to modulate the light falling on the painted surfaces. The 3-D structures in the displays were a matching set of wooden blocks. Across Mondrian displays, each corresponding facet had the same paint on its surface. We used only 6 chromatic, and 5 achromatic paints applied to 104 block facets. The 3-D blocks add shadows and multiple reflections not found in flat Mondrians. Both 3-D Mondrians were viewed simultaneously, side-by-side. We used two techniques to measure correlation of appearance with surface reflectance. First, observers made magnitude estimates of changes in the appearances of identical reflectances. Second, an author painted a watercolor of the 3-D Mondrians. The watercolor's reflectances quantified the changes in appearances. While constancy generalizations about illumination and reflectance hold for flat Mondrians, they do not for 3-D Mondrians. A constant paint does not exhibit perfect color constancy, but rather shows significant shifts in lightness, hue and chroma in response to the structure in the nonuniform illumination. Color appearance depends on the spatial information in both the illumination and the reflectances of objects. The spatial information of the quanta catch from the array of retinal receptors generates sensations that have variable correlation with surface reflectance. Models of appearance in humans need to calculate the departures from perfect constancy measured here. This article provides a dataset of measurements of color appearances for computational models of sensation. © 2014 McCann, Parraman and Rizzi

    Responses and preferences of salmon louse (Lepeophtheirus salmonis Krøyer 1836) copepodids to underwater artificial light sources

    Get PDF
    The ectoparasitic salmon louse (Lepeophtheirus salmonis, Krøyer 1836) remains a major disease problem and cost driver in commercial Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar L) farming and is also implicated in the decline of wild salmon stocks. The parasite feeds on mucus and blood causing skin damage on its host (i.e. leads to reduced welfare and disease resistance). Underwater lights (UL) are being used regularly in open-cage salmon aquaculture to delay maturation and increase feeding rates during the dark season. The aim of this study has therefore been to supply basic experimental data on the responses of the infectious copepodid stage of L. salmonis to discrete underwater light sources with different light qualities and intensities. The collective movement of a copepodid population in response to light sources was tested in a laboratory-based machine vision system using automated image processing. Copepodids always moved towards the light source even at low light intensities (1.5 × 10−3 μmol m−2 s−1) within a broad spectrum of visible light as well as near-UV. It is therefore plausible that subsea light sources frequently used in salmon farming under certain conditions can attract salmon lice copepodids and increase infection pressure. Moreover, the findings of our study support that light traps may be used to catch planktonic salmon lice. The actual effect of underwater light sources on the local distribution of salmon lice should be tested by controlled plankton sampling or monitoring in the vicinity of light sources compared to the surrounding water.publishedVersio

    Colour vision and background adaptation in a passerine bird, the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata)

    Get PDF
    Today, there is good knowledge of the physiological basis of bird colour vision and how mathematical models can be used to predict visual thresholds. However, we still know only little about how colour vision changes between different viewing conditions. This limits the understanding of how colour signalling is configured in habitats where the light of the illumination and the background may shift dramatically. I examined how colour discrimination in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) is affected by adaptation to different backgrounds. I trained finches in a two-alternative choice task, to choose between red discs displayed on backgrounds with di fferent colours. I found that discrimination thresholds correlate with stimulus contrast to the background. Thresholds are low, and in agreement with model predictions, for a background with a red colour similar to the discs. For the most contrasting green background, thresholds are about five times higher than this. Subsequently, I trained the finches for the detection of single discs on a grey background. Detection thresholds are about 2.5 to 3 times higher than discrimination thresholds. This study demonstrates close similarities in human and bird colour vision, and the quantitative data offer a new possibility to account for shifting viewing conditions in colour vision models

    Delayed cone-opponent signals in the luminance pathway

    Get PDF
    Cone signals in the luminance or achromatic pathway were investigated by measuring how the perceptual timing of M- or L-cone-detected flicker depended on temporal frequency and chromatic adaptation. Relative timings were measured, as a function of temporal frequency, by superimposing M- or L-cone-isolating flicker on "equichromatic" flicker (flicker of the same wavelength as the background) and asking observers to vary contrast and phase to cancel the perception of flicker. Measurements were made in four observers on up to 35 different backgrounds varying in wavelength and radiance. Observers showed substantial perceptual delays or advances of L- and M-cone flicker that varied systematically with cone class, background wavelength, and radiance. Delays were largest for M-cone-isolating flicker. Although complex, the results can be characterised by a surprisingly simple model in which the representations of L- and M-cone flicker are comprised not only of a fast copy of the flicker signal, but also of a slow copy that is delayed by roughly 30 ms and varies in strength and sign with both background wavelength and radiance. The delays, which are too large to be due to selective cone adaptation by the chromatic backgrounds, must arise postreceptorally. Clear evidence for the slow signals can also be found in physiological measurements of horizontal and magnocellular ganglion cells, thus placing the origin of the slow signals in the retina-most likely in an extended horizontal cell network. Luminance-equated stimuli chosen to isolate chromatic channels may inadvertently generate slow signals in the luminance channel

    Detection of fruit and the selection of primate visual pigments for color vision

    Get PDF
    Primates have X chromosome genes for cone photopigments with sensitivity maxima from 535 to 562 nm. Old World monkeys and apes (catarrhines) and the New World (platyrrhine) genus Alouatta have separate genes for 535-nm (medium wavelength; M) and 562-nm (long wavelength; L) pigments. These pigments, together with a 425-nm (short wavelength) pigment, permit trichromatic color vision. Other platyrrhines and prosimians have a single X chromosome gene but often with alleles for two or three M/L photopigments. Consequently, heterozygote females are trichromats, but males and homozygote females are dichromats. The criteria that affect the evolution of M/L alleles and maintain genetic polymorphism remain a puzzle, but selection for finding food may be important. We compare different types of color vision for detecting more than 100 plant species consumed by tamarins (Saguinus spp.) in Peru. There is evidence that both frequency-dependent selection on homozygotes and heterozygote advantage favor M/L polymorphism and that trichromatic color vision is most advantageous in dim light. Also, whereas the 562-nm allele is present in all species, the occurrence of 535- to 556-nm alleles varies between species. This variation probably arises because trichromatic color vision favors widely separated pigments and equal frequencies of 535/543- and 562-nm alleles, whereas in dichromats, long-wavelength pigment alleles are fitter

    Assessment of foveal cone photoreceptors in Stargardt's macular dystrophy using a small dot detection task

    Full text link
    We measured frequency-of-seeing curves for tiny (1.125 and 3.375 min arc) stimuli flashed briefly at absolute threshold to estimate the density of foveal cones in normals and in subjects with Stargardt's macular dystrophy. Foveal absolute thresholds for Stargardt's were elevated 1.5 log units over normal. Analysis using Poisson counting statistics indicated that the quantal absorption to stimulate individual cones was normal for Stargardt's but that effective optical density of individual cones was reduced by > 1 log. Numerical density of foveal cones was reduced 1 log unit for Stargardt's patients with acuities of 20/30-20/100.Peer Reviewedhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/30727/1/0000376.pd

    The time-course of colour vision

    Get PDF
    Four experiments are presented, each investigating temporal properties of colour vision processing in human observers. The first experiment replicates and extends an experiment by Stromeyer et al. (1991). We look for a phase difference between combined temporal modulations in orthogonal directions in colour space, which might null the often-claimed latency of signals originating from the short-wavelength sensitive cones (S-cones). We provide another estimate of the magnitude of this latency, and give evidence to suggest that it originates early in the chromatic pathway, before signals from S-cones are combined with those that receive opposed L- and M-cone input. In the second experiment we adapt observers to two stimuli that are matched in the mean and amplitude of modulation they offer to the cone classes and to the cardinal opponent mechanisms, but that differ in chromatic appearance, and hence their modulation of later colour mechanisms. Chromatic discrimination thresholds after adaptation to these two stimuli differ along intermediate directions in colour space, and we argue that these differences reveal the adaptation response of central colour mechanisms. In the third experiment we demonstrate similar adaptation using the same stimuli, measured with reaction times rather than thresholds. In the final experiment, we measure the degree to which colour constancy is achieved as a function of time in a simulated stimulus environment in which the illuminant changes periodically. We find that perfect constancy is not achieved instantaneously after an illuminant chromaticity shift and that constancy of colour appearance judgements increases over several seconds
    corecore