1,659 research outputs found

    Self-imitating Feedback Generation Using GAN for Computer-Assisted Pronunciation Training

    Full text link
    Self-imitating feedback is an effective and learner-friendly method for non-native learners in Computer-Assisted Pronunciation Training. Acoustic characteristics in native utterances are extracted and transplanted onto learner's own speech input, and given back to the learner as a corrective feedback. Previous works focused on speech conversion using prosodic transplantation techniques based on PSOLA algorithm. Motivated by the visual differences found in spectrograms of native and non-native speeches, we investigated applying GAN to generate self-imitating feedback by utilizing generator's ability through adversarial training. Because this mapping is highly under-constrained, we also adopt cycle consistency loss to encourage the output to preserve the global structure, which is shared by native and non-native utterances. Trained on 97,200 spectrogram images of short utterances produced by native and non-native speakers of Korean, the generator is able to successfully transform the non-native spectrogram input to a spectrogram with properties of self-imitating feedback. Furthermore, the transformed spectrogram shows segmental corrections that cannot be obtained by prosodic transplantation. Perceptual test comparing the self-imitating and correcting abilities of our method with the baseline PSOLA method shows that the generative approach with cycle consistency loss is promising

    Directions for the future of technology in pronunciation research and teaching

    Get PDF
    This paper reports on the role of technology in state-of-the-art pronunciation research and instruction, and makes concrete suggestions for future developments. The point of departure for this contribution is that the goal of second language (L2) pronunciation research and teaching should be enhanced comprehensibility and intelligibility as opposed to native-likeness. Three main areas are covered here. We begin with a presentation of advanced uses of pronunciation technology in research with a special focus on the expertise required to carry out even small-scale investigations. Next, we discuss the nature of data in pronunciation research, pointing to ways in which future work can build on advances in corpus research and crowdsourcing. Finally, we consider how these insights pave the way for researchers and developers working to create research-informed, computer-assisted pronunciation teaching resources. We conclude with predictions for future developments

    CAPT๋ฅผ ์œ„ํ•œ ๋ฐœ์Œ ๋ณ€์ด ๋ถ„์„ ๋ฐ CycleGAN ๊ธฐ๋ฐ˜ ํ”ผ๋“œ๋ฐฑ ์ƒ์„ฑ

    Get PDF
    ํ•™์œ„๋…ผ๋ฌธ(๋ฐ•์‚ฌ)--์„œ์šธ๋Œ€ํ•™๊ต ๋Œ€ํ•™์› :์ธ๋ฌธ๋Œ€ํ•™ ํ˜‘๋™๊ณผ์ • ์ธ์ง€๊ณผํ•™์ „๊ณต,2020. 2. ์ •๋ฏผํ™”.Despite the growing popularity in learning Korean as a foreign language and the rapid development in language learning applications, the existing computer-assisted pronunciation training (CAPT) systems in Korean do not utilize linguistic characteristics of non-native Korean speech. Pronunciation variations in non-native speech are far more diverse than those observed in native speech, which may pose a difficulty in combining such knowledge in an automatic system. Moreover, most of the existing methods rely on feature extraction results from signal processing, prosodic analysis, and natural language processing techniques. Such methods entail limitations since they necessarily depend on finding the right features for the task and the extraction accuracies. This thesis presents a new approach for corrective feedback generation in a CAPT system, in which pronunciation variation patterns and linguistic correlates with accentedness are analyzed and combined with a deep neural network approach, so that feature engineering efforts are minimized while maintaining the linguistically important factors for the corrective feedback generation task. Investigations on non-native Korean speech characteristics in contrast with those of native speakers, and their correlation with accentedness judgement show that both segmental and prosodic variations are important factors in a Korean CAPT system. The present thesis argues that the feedback generation task can be interpreted as a style transfer problem, and proposes to evaluate the idea using generative adversarial network. A corrective feedback generation model is trained on 65,100 read utterances by 217 non-native speakers of 27 mother tongue backgrounds. The features are automatically learnt in an unsupervised way in an auxiliary classifier CycleGAN setting, in which the generator learns to map a foreign accented speech to native speech distributions. In order to inject linguistic knowledge into the network, an auxiliary classifier is trained so that the feedback also identifies the linguistic error types that were defined in the first half of the thesis. The proposed approach generates a corrected version the speech using the learners own voice, outperforming the conventional Pitch-Synchronous Overlap-and-Add method.์™ธ๊ตญ์–ด๋กœ์„œ์˜ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ๊ต์œก์— ๋Œ€ํ•œ ๊ด€์‹ฌ์ด ๊ณ ์กฐ๋˜์–ด ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ•™์Šต์ž์˜ ์ˆ˜๊ฐ€ ํฌ๊ฒŒ ์ฆ๊ฐ€ํ•˜๊ณ  ์žˆ์œผ๋ฉฐ, ์Œ์„ฑ์–ธ์–ด์ฒ˜๋ฆฌ ๊ธฐ์ˆ ์„ ์ ์šฉํ•œ ์ปดํ“จํ„ฐ ๊ธฐ๋ฐ˜ ๋ฐœ์Œ ๊ต์œก(Computer-Assisted Pronunciation Training; CAPT) ์–ดํ”Œ๋ฆฌ์ผ€์ด์…˜์— ๋Œ€ํ•œ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ ๋˜ํ•œ ์ ๊ทน์ ์œผ๋กœ ์ด๋ฃจ์–ด์ง€๊ณ  ์žˆ๋‹ค. ๊ทธ๋Ÿผ์—๋„ ๋ถˆ๊ตฌํ•˜๊ณ  ํ˜„์กดํ•˜๋Š” ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ๋งํ•˜๊ธฐ ๊ต์œก ์‹œ์Šคํ…œ์€ ์™ธ๊ตญ์ธ์˜ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด์— ๋Œ€ํ•œ ์–ธ์–ดํ•™์  ํŠน์ง•์„ ์ถฉ๋ถ„ํžˆ ํ™œ์šฉํ•˜์ง€ ์•Š๊ณ  ์žˆ์œผ๋ฉฐ, ์ตœ์‹  ์–ธ์–ด์ฒ˜๋ฆฌ ๊ธฐ์ˆ  ๋˜ํ•œ ์ ์šฉ๋˜์ง€ ์•Š๊ณ  ์žˆ๋Š” ์‹ค์ •์ด๋‹ค. ๊ฐ€๋Šฅํ•œ ์›์ธ์œผ๋กœ์จ๋Š” ์™ธ๊ตญ์ธ ๋ฐœํ™” ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ˜„์ƒ์— ๋Œ€ํ•œ ๋ถ„์„์ด ์ถฉ๋ถ„ํ•˜๊ฒŒ ์ด๋ฃจ์–ด์ง€์ง€ ์•Š์•˜๋‹ค๋Š” ์ , ๊ทธ๋ฆฌ๊ณ  ๊ด€๋ จ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๊ฐ€ ์žˆ์–ด๋„ ์ด๋ฅผ ์ž๋™ํ™”๋œ ์‹œ์Šคํ…œ์— ๋ฐ˜์˜ํ•˜๊ธฐ์—๋Š” ๊ณ ๋„ํ™”๋œ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๊ฐ€ ํ•„์š”ํ•˜๋‹ค๋Š” ์ ์ด ์žˆ๋‹ค. ๋ฟ๋งŒ ์•„๋‹ˆ๋ผ CAPT ๊ธฐ์ˆ  ์ „๋ฐ˜์ ์œผ๋กœ๋Š” ์‹ ํ˜ธ์ฒ˜๋ฆฌ, ์šด์œจ ๋ถ„์„, ์ž์—ฐ์–ด์ฒ˜๋ฆฌ ๊ธฐ๋ฒ•๊ณผ ๊ฐ™์€ ํŠน์ง• ์ถ”์ถœ์— ์˜์กดํ•˜๊ณ  ์žˆ์–ด์„œ ์ ํ•ฉํ•œ ํŠน์ง•์„ ์ฐพ๊ณ  ์ด๋ฅผ ์ •ํ™•ํ•˜๊ฒŒ ์ถ”์ถœํ•˜๋Š” ๋ฐ์— ๋งŽ์€ ์‹œ๊ฐ„๊ณผ ๋…ธ๋ ฅ์ด ํ•„์š”ํ•œ ์‹ค์ •์ด๋‹ค. ์ด๋Š” ์ตœ์‹  ๋”ฅ๋Ÿฌ๋‹ ๊ธฐ๋ฐ˜ ์–ธ์–ด์ฒ˜๋ฆฌ ๊ธฐ์ˆ ์„ ํ™œ์šฉํ•จ์œผ๋กœ์จ ์ด ๊ณผ์ • ๋˜ํ•œ ๋ฐœ์ „์˜ ์—ฌ์ง€๊ฐ€ ๋งŽ๋‹ค๋Š” ๋ฐ”๋ฅผ ์‹œ์‚ฌํ•œ๋‹ค. ๋”ฐ๋ผ์„œ ๋ณธ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๋Š” ๋จผ์ € CAPT ์‹œ์Šคํ…œ ๊ฐœ๋ฐœ์— ์žˆ์–ด ๋ฐœ์Œ ๋ณ€์ด ์–‘์ƒ๊ณผ ์–ธ์–ดํ•™์  ์ƒ๊ด€๊ด€๊ณ„๋ฅผ ๋ถ„์„ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ์™ธ๊ตญ์ธ ํ™”์ž๋“ค์˜ ๋‚ญ๋…์ฒด ๋ณ€์ด ์–‘์ƒ๊ณผ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ์›์–ด๋ฏผ ํ™”์ž๋“ค์˜ ๋‚ญ๋…์ฒด ๋ณ€์ด ์–‘์ƒ์„ ๋Œ€์กฐํ•˜๊ณ  ์ฃผ์š”ํ•œ ๋ณ€์ด๋ฅผ ํ™•์ธํ•œ ํ›„, ์ƒ๊ด€๊ด€๊ณ„ ๋ถ„์„์„ ํ†ตํ•˜์—ฌ ์˜์‚ฌ์†Œํ†ต์— ์˜ํ–ฅ์„ ๋ฏธ์น˜๋Š” ์ค‘์š”๋„๋ฅผ ํŒŒ์•…ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ๊ทธ ๊ฒฐ๊ณผ, ์ข…์„ฑ ์‚ญ์ œ์™€ 3์ค‘ ๋Œ€๋ฆฝ์˜ ํ˜ผ๋™, ์ดˆ๋ถ„์ ˆ ๊ด€๋ จ ์˜ค๋ฅ˜๊ฐ€ ๋ฐœ์ƒํ•  ๊ฒฝ์šฐ ํ”ผ๋“œ๋ฐฑ ์ƒ์„ฑ์— ์šฐ์„ ์ ์œผ๋กœ ๋ฐ˜์˜ํ•˜๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์ด ํ•„์š”ํ•˜๋‹ค๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์ด ํ™•์ธ๋˜์—ˆ๋‹ค. ๊ต์ •๋œ ํ”ผ๋“œ๋ฐฑ์„ ์ž๋™์œผ๋กœ ์ƒ์„ฑํ•˜๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์€ CAPT ์‹œ์Šคํ…œ์˜ ์ค‘์š”ํ•œ ๊ณผ์ œ ์ค‘ ํ•˜๋‚˜์ด๋‹ค. ๋ณธ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๋Š” ์ด ๊ณผ์ œ๊ฐ€ ๋ฐœํ™”์˜ ์Šคํƒ€์ผ ๋ณ€ํ™”์˜ ๋ฌธ์ œ๋กœ ํ•ด์„์ด ๊ฐ€๋Šฅํ•˜๋‹ค๊ณ  ๋ณด์•˜์œผ๋ฉฐ, ์ƒ์„ฑ์  ์ ๋Œ€ ์‹ ๊ฒฝ๋ง (Cycle-consistent Generative Adversarial Network; CycleGAN) ๊ตฌ์กฐ์—์„œ ๋ชจ๋ธ๋งํ•˜๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์„ ์ œ์•ˆํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. GAN ๋„คํŠธ์›Œํฌ์˜ ์ƒ์„ฑ๋ชจ๋ธ์€ ๋น„์›์–ด๋ฏผ ๋ฐœํ™”์˜ ๋ถ„ํฌ์™€ ์›์–ด๋ฏผ ๋ฐœํ™” ๋ถ„ํฌ์˜ ๋งคํ•‘์„ ํ•™์Šตํ•˜๋ฉฐ, Cycle consistency ์†์‹คํ•จ์ˆ˜๋ฅผ ์‚ฌ์šฉํ•จ์œผ๋กœ์จ ๋ฐœํ™”๊ฐ„ ์ „๋ฐ˜์ ์ธ ๊ตฌ์กฐ๋ฅผ ์œ ์ง€ํ•จ๊ณผ ๋™์‹œ์— ๊ณผ๋„ํ•œ ๊ต์ •์„ ๋ฐฉ์ง€ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ๋ณ„๋„์˜ ํŠน์ง• ์ถ”์ถœ ๊ณผ์ •์ด ์—†์ด ํ•„์š”ํ•œ ํŠน์ง•๋“ค์ด CycleGAN ํ”„๋ ˆ์ž„์›Œํฌ์—์„œ ๋ฌด๊ฐ๋… ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์œผ๋กœ ์Šค์Šค๋กœ ํ•™์Šต๋˜๋Š” ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์œผ๋กœ, ์–ธ์–ด ํ™•์žฅ์ด ์šฉ์ดํ•œ ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์ด๋‹ค. ์–ธ์–ดํ•™์  ๋ถ„์„์—์„œ ๋“œ๋Ÿฌ๋‚œ ์ฃผ์š”ํ•œ ๋ณ€์ด๋“ค ๊ฐ„์˜ ์šฐ์„ ์ˆœ์œ„๋Š” Auxiliary Classifier CycleGAN ๊ตฌ์กฐ์—์„œ ๋ชจ๋ธ๋งํ•˜๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์„ ์ œ์•ˆํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ์ด ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์€ ๊ธฐ์กด์˜ CycleGAN์— ์ง€์‹์„ ์ ‘๋ชฉ์‹œ์ผœ ํ”ผ๋“œ๋ฐฑ ์Œ์„ฑ์„ ์ƒ์„ฑํ•จ๊ณผ ๋™์‹œ์— ํ•ด๋‹น ํ”ผ๋“œ๋ฐฑ์ด ์–ด๋–ค ์œ ํ˜•์˜ ์˜ค๋ฅ˜์ธ์ง€ ๋ถ„๋ฅ˜ํ•˜๋Š” ๋ฌธ์ œ๋ฅผ ์ˆ˜ํ–‰ํ•œ๋‹ค. ์ด๋Š” ๋„๋ฉ”์ธ ์ง€์‹์ด ๊ต์ • ํ”ผ๋“œ๋ฐฑ ์ƒ์„ฑ ๋‹จ๊ณ„๊นŒ์ง€ ์œ ์ง€๋˜๊ณ  ํ†ต์ œ๊ฐ€ ๊ฐ€๋Šฅํ•˜๋‹ค๋Š” ์žฅ์ ์ด ์žˆ๋‹ค๋Š” ๋ฐ์— ๊ทธ ์˜์˜๊ฐ€ ์žˆ๋‹ค. ๋ณธ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ์—์„œ ์ œ์•ˆํ•œ ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์„ ํ‰๊ฐ€ํ•˜๊ธฐ ์œ„ํ•ด์„œ 27๊ฐœ์˜ ๋ชจ๊ตญ์–ด๋ฅผ ๊ฐ–๋Š” 217๋ช…์˜ ์œ ์˜๋ฏธ ์–ดํœ˜ ๋ฐœํ™” 65,100๊ฐœ๋กœ ํ”ผ๋“œ๋ฐฑ ์ž๋™ ์ƒ์„ฑ ๋ชจ๋ธ์„ ํ›ˆ๋ จํ•˜๊ณ , ๊ฐœ์„  ์—ฌ๋ถ€ ๋ฐ ์ •๋„์— ๋Œ€ํ•œ ์ง€๊ฐ ํ‰๊ฐ€๋ฅผ ์ˆ˜ํ–‰ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ์ œ์•ˆ๋œ ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์„ ์‚ฌ์šฉํ•˜์˜€์„ ๋•Œ ํ•™์Šต์ž ๋ณธ์ธ์˜ ๋ชฉ์†Œ๋ฆฌ๋ฅผ ์œ ์ง€ํ•œ ์ฑ„ ๊ต์ •๋œ ๋ฐœ์Œ์œผ๋กœ ๋ณ€ํ™˜ํ•˜๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์ด ๊ฐ€๋Šฅํ•˜๋ฉฐ, ์ „ํ†ต์ ์ธ ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์ธ ์Œ๋†’์ด ๋™๊ธฐ์‹ ์ค‘์ฒฉ๊ฐ€์‚ฐ (Pitch-Synchronous Overlap-and-Add) ์•Œ๊ณ ๋ฆฌ์ฆ˜์„ ์‚ฌ์šฉํ•˜๋Š” ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•์— ๋น„ํ•ด ์ƒ๋Œ€ ๊ฐœ์„ ๋ฅ  16.67%์ด ํ™•์ธ๋˜์—ˆ๋‹ค.Chapter 1. Introduction 1 1.1. Motivation 1 1.1.1. An Overview of CAPT Systems 3 1.1.2. Survey of existing Korean CAPT Systems 5 1.2. Problem Statement 7 1.3. Thesis Structure 7 Chapter 2. Pronunciation Analysis of Korean Produced by Chinese 9 2.1. Comparison between Korean and Chinese 11 2.1.1. Phonetic and Syllable Structure Comparisons 11 2.1.2. Phonological Comparisons 14 2.2. Related Works 16 2.3. Proposed Analysis Method 19 2.3.1. Corpus 19 2.3.2. Transcribers and Agreement Rates 22 2.4. Salient Pronunciation Variations 22 2.4.1. Segmental Variation Patterns 22 2.4.1.1. Discussions 25 2.4.2. Phonological Variation Patterns 26 2.4.1.2. Discussions 27 2.5. Summary 29 Chapter 3. Correlation Analysis of Pronunciation Variations and Human Evaluation 30 3.1. Related Works 31 3.1.1. Criteria used in L2 Speech 31 3.1.2. Criteria used in L2 Korean Speech 32 3.2. Proposed Human Evaluation Method 36 3.2.1. Reading Prompt Design 36 3.2.2. Evaluation Criteria Design 37 3.2.3. Raters and Agreement Rates 40 3.3. Linguistic Factors Affecting L2 Korean Accentedness 41 3.3.1. Pearsons Correlation Analysis 41 3.3.2. Discussions 42 3.3.3. Implications for Automatic Feedback Generation 44 3.4. Summary 45 Chapter 4. Corrective Feedback Generation for CAPT 46 4.1. Related Works 46 4.1.1. Prosody Transplantation 47 4.1.2. Recent Speech Conversion Methods 49 4.1.3. Evaluation of Corrective Feedback 50 4.2. Proposed Method: Corrective Feedback as a Style Transfer 51 4.2.1. Speech Analysis at Spectral Domain 53 4.2.2. Self-imitative Learning 55 4.2.3. An Analogy: CAPT System and GAN Architecture 57 4.3. Generative Adversarial Networks 59 4.3.1. Conditional GAN 61 4.3.2. CycleGAN 62 4.4. Experiment 63 4.4.1. Corpus 64 4.4.2. Baseline Implementation 65 4.4.3. Adversarial Training Implementation 65 4.4.4. Spectrogram-to-Spectrogram Training 66 4.5. Results and Evaluation 69 4.5.1. Spectrogram Generation Results 69 4.5.2. Perceptual Evaluation 70 4.5.3. Discussions 72 4.6. Summary 74 Chapter 5. Integration of Linguistic Knowledge in an Auxiliary Classifier CycleGAN for Feedback Generation 75 5.1. Linguistic Class Selection 75 5.2. Auxiliary Classifier CycleGAN Design 77 5.3. Experiment and Results 80 5.3.1. Corpus 80 5.3.2. Feature Annotations 81 5.3.3. Experiment Setup 81 5.3.4. Results 82 5.4. Summary 84 Chapter 6. Conclusion 86 6.1. Thesis Results 86 6.2. Thesis Contributions 88 6.3. Recommendations for Future Work 89 Bibliography 91 Appendix 107 Abstract in Korean 117 Acknowledgments 120Docto

    On the (un)conditionality of automatic attitude activation: the valence proportion effect

    Get PDF
    Affective priming studies have shown that participants are faster to pronounce affectively polarized target words that are preceded by affectively congruent prime words than affectively polarized target words that are preceded by affectively incongruent prime words. We examined whether affective priming of naming responses depends on the valence proportion (i.e., the proportion of stimuli that are affectively polarized). In one group of participants, experimental trials were embedded in a context of filler trials that consisted of affectively polarized stimulus materials (i.e., high valence proportion condition). In a second group, the same set of experimental trials was embedded in a context of filler trials consisting of neutral stimuli (i.e., low valence proportion condition). Results showed that affective priming of naming responses was significantly stronger in the high valence proportion condition than in the low valence proportion condition. We conclude that (a) subtle aspects of the procedure can influence affective priming of naming responses, (b) finding affective priming of naming responses does not allow for the conclusion that affective stimulus processing is unconditional, and (c) affective stimulus processing depends on selective attention for affective stimulus information

    The locus of post-lexical semantic matching effects on semantic priming: biasing a binary response or a binary decision?

    Get PDF
    Two hundred introductory level students at the University of Wisconsin-LaCrosse and at Iowa State University served as participants. Forty participants each were assigned to each level of the between-subjects manipulation of type of task (standard lexical decision, standard pronunciation, single-response lexical decision, keypress go/no go and pronunciation go/no go). Type of priming (forward, mediated, and backward) served as a within-subjects manipulation;The pattern of priming across tasks failed to support predictions derived from Neely and Keefe\u27s three-process theory (1989), which predicts mediated priming only for pronunciation and backward priming only for lexical decision. The data showed a reliable mediated priming effect for all tasks except pronunciation. Pronunciation did not show a reliable backward priming effect. Reliable forward priming was found for all tasks. The data were inconclusive regarding the locus of the effects of the three processes outlined by Neely and Keefe. Theories of semantic priming need to be reexamined to incorporate these unexpected patterns

    The Effect of Bilingual Proficiency in Indian English on Bilabial Plosive

    Get PDF
    Background: Bilingual speech production studies have highlighted that level of proficiency influences the acoustic-phonetic representation of phonemes in both languages (MacKay, Flege, Piske, & Schirru 2001; Zรกrate-Sรกndez, 2015). The results for bilingual speech production reveal that proficient/early bilinguals produce distinct acoustic properties for the same phoneme in each language, whereas less proficient/late bilinguals produce acoustic properties for a phoneme that is closer to the native language (Flege et al., 2003; Fowler et al., 2008). Acoustic-phonetic studies for Hindi (L1) and Indian English (L2) for bilingual speakers have been understudied, and the level of proficiency has not been considered in Hindi and Indian English bilingual speakers. The present study aimed to measure the acoustic differences produced by bilingual speakers of varying proficiencies for Indian English on bilabial plosive and determine how the bilabial plosives are different from American English bilabial plosives. Methods: The sample size for this study was twenty-four. However, only twenty participants (eleven females) between the ages of eighteen and fifty, with normal speech and hearing, were recruited. The lack of recruitment of four more participants was due to the inability to find bilingual speakers who spoke Hindi as their first language and Indian English as their second language and COVID-19 restrictions imposed on recruitment (n=4). The participants were divided into three groups based on language and proficiency: a monolingual American English group, a proficient bilingual Hindi-Indian English group, and a less-proficient bilingual Hindi-Indian English group. The bilinguals were divided into a proficient and less proficient group based on the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (Marian, Blumenfeld, & Kaushanskaya, 2007). Following the screening, participants took part in a Nonword Repetition Task. Data were analyzed using Praat and Voice Sauce software. A linear mixed-effects model using R statistics was used for the statistical analysis. Results: Data from 20 participants (seven proficient bilingual speakers, five less-proficient bilingual speakers, and eight monolingual speakers) were included in the data analysis. Approximately four thousand repetitions were evaluated across the remaining participants. There were no significant main effects across the four dependent variables, but there was an interaction effect between group and phoneme on two dependent variables. The closure duration for proficient bilingual speakers compared to less-proficient bilingual speakers were significantly different between the voiceless unaspirated bilabial plosive (VLE) and voiceless aspirated bilabial plosive (VLH), as well as voiced unaspirated bilabial plosive (VE) and voiced aspirated bilabial plosive (VH). For spectral tilt, there was a significant difference between the VLE and VLH for proficient bilingual speakers compared to less proficient bilingual speakers. Discussion: The results of this study suggest that proficient bilingual speakers have a faster rate of speech in both their first language and second language. Therefore, it is difficult to provide information on whether this group has separate acoustic-phonetic characteristics for each phoneme for each language. In contrast, the less-proficient bilingual speakers seem to have a unidirectional relationship (i.e., first language influences the second language). Furthermore, the results of the acoustic characteristics for the control group i.e., monolingual American English speakers suggest that they may have acoustic-phonetic characteristics that represent a single acoustic-phonetic representation of bilabial plosive with their voicing contrast

    A Sound Approach to Language Matters: In Honor of Ocke-Schwen Bohn

    Get PDF
    The contributions in this Festschrift were written by Ockeโ€™s current and former PhD-students, colleagues and research collaborators. The Festschrift is divided into six sections, moving from the smallest building blocks of language, through gradually expanding objects of linguistic inquiry to the highest levels of description - all of which have formed a part of Ockeโ€™s career, in connection with his teaching and/or his academic productions: โ€œSegmentsโ€, โ€œPerception of Accentโ€, โ€œBetween Sounds and Graphemesโ€, โ€œProsodyโ€, โ€œMorphology and Syntaxโ€ and โ€œSecond Language Acquisitionโ€.ย Each one of these illustrates a sound approach to language matters

    Phonetic detail in the developing lexicon

    Get PDF
    Although infants show remarkable sensitivity to linguistically relevant phonetic variation in speech, young children sometimes appear not to make use of this sensitivity. Here, children's knowledge of the sound-forms of familiar words was assessed using a visual fixation task. Dutch 19-month-olds were shown pairs of pictures and heard correct pronunciations and mispronunciations of familiar words naming one of the pictures. Mispronunciations were word-initial in Experiment 1 and word-medial in Experiment 2, and in both experiments involved substituting one segment with [d] (a common sound in Dutch) or [g] (a rare sound). In both experiments, word recognition performance was better for correct pronunciations than for mispronunciations involving either substituted consonant. These effects did not depend upon children's knowledge of lexical or nonlexical phonological neighbors of the tested words. The results indicate the encoding of phonetic detail in words at 19 months

    Adult cochlear implant users versus typical hearing persons: an automatic analysis of acousticโ€“prosodic pparameters

    Get PDF
    Purpose: The aim of this study was to investigate the speech prosody of postlingually deaf cochlear implant (CI) users compared with control speakers without hearing or speech impairment. Method: Speech recordings of 74 CI users (37 males and 37 females) and 72 age-balanced control speakers (36 males and 36 females) are considered. All participants are German native speakers and read Der Nordwind und die Sonne (The North Wind and the Sun), a standard text in pathological speech analysis and phonetic transcriptions. Automatic acoustic analysis is performed considering pitch, loudness, and duration features, including speech rate and rhythm. Results: In general, duration and rhythm features differ between CI users and control speakers. CI users read slower and have a lower voiced segment ratio compared with control speakers. A lower voiced ratio goes along with a prolongation of the voiced segments' duration in male and with a prolongation of pauses in female CI users. Rhythm features in CI users have higher variability in the duration of vowels and consonants than in control speakers. The use of bilateral CIs showed no advantages concerning speech prosody features in comparison to unilateral use of CI. Conclusions: Even after cochlear implantation and rehabilitation, the speech of postlingually deaf adults deviates from the speech of control speakers, which might be due to changed auditory feedback. We suggest considering changes in temporal aspects of speech in future rehabilitation strategies
    • โ€ฆ
    corecore