530 research outputs found

    Polar Bears: A Complete Guide to Their Biology and Behavior, by Andrew E. Derocher

    Get PDF

    PÀrilik vÀhk: diagnoos kinnitatud 23 aastat hiljem

    Get PDF
    1992. aasta juunikuu Eesti Arsti rubriigis „Kogemuste vahetamine ja kasuistika“ ilmus Evi Hindi ja Maret Purde artikkel „PĂ€rilik vĂ€hk: sugupuu uurimus“. Artiklis toodi pĂ€riliku vĂ€hi nĂ€idetena kahe suguvĂ”sa lugu: ĂŒhes perekonnas esines mitmetel lĂ€hisugulastel erinevaid pahaloomulisi kasvajaid, eelkĂ”ige jĂ€mesoolevĂ€hki, ning teises perekonnas rinna- ja maovĂ€hki. Kirjutis lĂ”ppes soovitusega, et lisaks tervete pereliikmete profĂŒlaktilisele uurimisele oleks „tulevikus ka geenianalĂŒĂŒs hĂ€davajalik” (1).2015. aasta septembris tuli TÜ Kliinikumi geneetikakeskusesse arsti vastuvĂ”tule 48aastane meespatsient, kaasas eelmainitud artikli koopia (vt foto 1). Patsient oli pĂ”denud 39 ja 41 aasta vanuses jĂ€mesoolevĂ€hki ning talle oli tehtud jĂ€mesoole osaline resektsioon. Kliinilise pildi ja pereanamneesi alusel oli diagnoositud Lynchi sĂŒndroomi ehk pĂ€rilikku mittepolĂŒpoosset jĂ€mesoolevĂ€hki, kuid geeniuuringuid ei olnud pereliikmetele seni tehtud. Aastatetaguses Eesti Arstis oli kirjeldatud tema isapoolset suguvĂ”sa. Patsiendi sooviks oli teha geeniuuringud, et leida ĂŒles perekonnas esinevate vĂ€hkkasvajate pĂ”hjus.Eesti Arst 2016; 95(9):610–61

    Fission–fusion populations

    Get PDF

    Beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, Distribution and Survey Effort in the Gulf of Alaska

    Get PDF
    Beluga, Delphinapterus leucas, distribution in the Gulf of Alaska and adjacent inside waters was examined through a review of surveys conducted as far back as 1936. Although beluga sightings have occurred on almost every marine mammal survey in northern Cook Inlet (over 20 surveys reported here), beluga sightings have been rare outside the inlet in the Gulf of Alaska. More than 150,000 km of dedicated survey effort in the Gulf of Alaska resulted in sightings of over 23,000 individual cetaceans, of which only 4 beluga sightings (5 individuals) occurred. In addition, nearly 100,000 individual cetaceans were reported in the Platforms of Opportunity database; yet, of these, only 5 sightings (39 individuals) were belugas. Furthermore, approximately 19 beluga sightings (>260 individuals), possibly including resightings, have been reported without information on effort or other cetacean sightings. Of the 28 sightings of belugas outside of Cook Inlet, 9 were near Kodiak Island, 10 were in or near Prince William Sound, 8 were in Yakutat Bay, and 1 anomalous sighting was well south of the Gulf. These sightings support archaeological and commercial harvest evidence indicating the only persistent group of belugas in the Gulf of Alaska occurs in Cook Inlet

    Meaningful Gesture in Monkeys? Investigating whether Mandrills Create Social Culture

    Get PDF
    BACKGROUND: Human societies exhibit a rich array of gestures with cultural origins. Often these gestures are found exclusively in local populations, where their meaning has been crafted by a community into a shared convention. In nonhuman primates like African monkeys, little evidence exists for such culturally-conventionalized gestures. METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS: Here I report a striking gesture unique to a single community of mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) among nineteen studied across North America, Africa, and Europe. The gesture was found within a community of 23 mandrills where individuals old and young, female and male covered their eyes with their hands for periods which could exceed 30 min, often while simultaneously raising their elbow prominently into the air. This 'Eye covering' gesture has been performed within the community for a decade, enduring deaths, removals, and births, and it persists into the present. Differential responses to Eye covering versus controls suggested that the gesture might have a locally-respected meaning, potentially functioning over a distance to inhibit interruptions as a 'do not disturb' sign operates. CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE: The creation of this gesture by monkeys suggests that the ability to cultivate shared meanings using novel manual acts may be distributed more broadly beyond the human species. Although logistically difficult with primates, the translocation of gesturers between communities remains critical to experimentally establishing the possible cultural origin and transmission of nonhuman gestures

    Onkoloogilised konsultatsioonid meditsiinigeneetikas: nÀidustused ja kliiniline praktika

    Get PDF
    Pahaloomulised kasvajad on ĂŒks sagedamini esinevaid terviseprobleeme tĂ€napĂ€eva ĂŒhiskonnas. Tulenevalt sellest tegelevad ka meditsiinigeneetikud oma töös onkogeneetiliste konsultatsioonidega jĂ€rjest enam. Et suurenenud nĂ”udlusega kaasas kĂ€ia, on arenenud ka uuringuvĂ”imalused ja metoodikad. Artikli eesmĂ€rk on anda ĂŒlevaade onkoloogiliste patsientide uuringuvĂ”imalustest TÜ Kliinikumi ĂŒhendlabori kliinilise geneetika keskuses Tartus ja Tallinnas. Lisaks on kĂ€sitletud patsientide konsultatsioonile suunamist, uuringutaktikat lĂ€htuvalt kasvaja tĂŒĂŒbist ning sellest, kas tegemist on iduliini vĂ”i somaatilise muutusega, ning uuringutest saadavat vĂ”imalikku kasu nii patsiendi kui ka tema lĂ€hisugulaste ja ka ĂŒhiskonna tasandil. Tegu on koolitusartikliga, milles on antud ĂŒlevaade kliinilise geneetika keskuse kliinilisest tööst ja kogemusest ning soovitusi, lĂ€htudes teaduspĂ”histest diagnostika- ning ravijuhenditest

    Do Wild Polar Bears (Ursus maritimus) Use Tools When Hunting Walruses (Odobenus rosmarus)?

    Get PDF
    Since the late 1700s, reports of polar bears (Ursus maritimus) using tools (i.e., pieces of ice or stones) to kill walruses (Odobenus rosmarus) have been passed on verbally to explorers and naturalists by their Inuit guides, based on local traditional ecological knowledge (TEK) as well as accounts of direct observations or interpretations of tracks in the snow made by the Inuit hunters who reported them. To assess the possibility that polar bears may occasionally use tools to hunt walruses in the wild, we summarize 1) observations described to early explorers and naturalists by Inuit hunters about polar bears using tools, 2) more recent documentation in the literature from Inuit hunters and scientists, and 3) recent observations of a polar bear in a zoo spontaneously using tools to access a novel food source. These observations and previously published experiments on brown bears (Ursus arctos) confirm that, in captivity, polar and brown bears are both capable of conceptualizing the use of a tool to obtain a food source that would otherwise not be accessible. Based on the information from all our sources, this may occasionally also have been the case in the wild. We suggest that possible tool use by polar bears in the wild is infrequent and mainly limited to hunting walruses because of their large size, difficulty to kill, and their possession of potentially lethal weapons for both their own defense and the direct attack of a predator. Depuis la fin des annĂ©es 1700, des signalements d’ours polaires (Ursus maritimus) se servant d’outils (comme des morceaux de glace ou des pierres) pour tuer des morses (Odobenus rosmarus) ont Ă©tĂ© communiquĂ©s verbalement par des guides inuits Ă  divers explorateurs et naturalistes. Les guides en question se fondaient sur les connaissances Ă©cologiques traditionnelles (CET) locales de mĂȘme que sur les interprĂ©tations de traces dans la neige ou les rĂ©cits d’observations directes des chasseurs inuits ayant fait les signalements. Pour Ă©valuer la possibilitĂ© que les ours polaires puissent parfois se servir d’outils pour chasser les morses en milieu sauvage, nous rĂ©sumons : 1) les observations dĂ©crites aux premiers explorateurs et naturalistes par les chasseurs inuits au sujet de l’utilisation d’outils par les ours polaires; 2) la documentation rĂ©cente attribuable aux chasseurs inuits et aux scientifiques; et 3) les rĂ©centes observations de l’ours polaire d’un zoo se servant d’outils spontanĂ©ment pour avoir accĂšs Ă  une nouvelle source de nourriture. Ces observations, alliĂ©es Ă  des expĂ©riences publiĂ©es au sujet d’ours bruns (Ursus arctos), permettent de confirmer qu’en captivitĂ©, tant les ours bruns que les ours polaires sont capables de conceptualiser l’utilisation d’un outil pour se procurer de la nourriture qui ne serait autrement pas accessible. D’aprĂšs les renseignements prĂ©levĂ©s auprĂšs de toutes nos sources, cela aurait aussi pu ĂȘtre occasionnellement le cas en milieu sauvage. Nous suggĂ©rons que l’utilisation possible d’outils par les ours polaires en milieu sauvage n’est pas frĂ©quente et qu’elle est surtout limitĂ©e Ă  la chasse au morse en raison de la grande taille de cette espĂšce, de la difficultĂ© Ă  l’abattre et des armes potentiellement mortelles qu’elle possĂšde, tant pour se dĂ©fendre que pour attaquer un prĂ©dateur directement.&nbsp
    • 

    corecore