3,418 research outputs found

    Team Building Interventions in Sport: A Meta-Analysis

    Get PDF
    Team building has been defined as “a method of helping the group to (a) increase effectiveness, (b) satisfy the needs of its members, or (c) improve work conditions” (Brawley & Paskevich, 1997, p. 13). The primary purpose of the present study was to conduct a meta-analysis of studies using team building interventions in sport teams. A secondary purpose was to examine the influence of various moderator variables: study design (quasi- or non-experimental), source of data (published or unpublished), type of intervention (goal setting, interpersonal relations, adventure programs, omnibus), delivery (direct or indirect), length of intervention (\u3c2 weeks, 2-20 weeks, \u3e20 weeks), gender (male, female, mixed), sport type (interactive, individual, combination), and finally, skill level (high school, intercollegiate, post-intercollegiate). The dependent variables examined were: cohesion (task and social), performance, enhanced cognitions, roles, and anxiety. Standard literature searches produced 17 studies containing a total of 180 effect sizes. The effect sizes were subjected to the corrections suggested by Hedges and Olkin (1985). The overall average Hedges g was .427 (p \u3c.001). Subsequent analyses indicated: (a) a moderate positive effect for both quasi- (.408, p \u3c .001) and non-experimental (.474, p \u3c .001) designs; (b) a moderate positive effect for published (.385, p \u3c .001), along with a moderate to large positive effect for unpublished (.539, p \u3c .001) studies; (c) a large positive effect for goal setting interventions (.714, p \u3c .001) compared to a small positive effect for interventions incorporating a combination of strategies (.161, p \u3e .05), and moderate positive effects for targeting interpersonal relations (.486, p \u3e .05) or using adventure programs (.471, p \u3c .001); (d) moderate positive effects for both direct (.446, p \u3c .001) and indirect protocols for intervention delivery (.414, p \u3c .001); (e) a small - iii - positive effect for interventions lasting \u3c 2 weeks (.106, p \u3e .05), versus moderate positive effects for interventions ranging between 2-20 weeks (.499, p \u3c .001) and lasting longer than 2 weeks (.564, p\u3c .001); (f) a moderate positive effect for both males (.525, p \u3c .001 ) and females (.458, p \u3c .001), and a large positive effect for groups of mixed gender (.712, p \u3c .01); (g) a small positive effect in team sports (.159, p \u3e .05), and a moderate to large positive effect in individual sports (.673, p \u3c .001) and in studies examining a combination of sports (.712, p \u3c.01); and (h) a small positive effect for high school (.240, p \u3e .05) and post-intercollegiate groups (.218, p \u3e.05), and a moderate positive effect for intercollegiate groups (.482, p \u3c.001). With regard to the dependent variable categories, small positive effects were found for social cohesion (.214, p\u3c .01), task cohesion (.263, p \u3e .05), and anxiety (.165, p \u3e .05), whereas large positive effects were found for performance (712, p \u3c.001), enhanced cognitions (.799, p \u3c .001), and roles (.789, p \u3e .05). The results are discussed in relation to their contribution to the theory and practice of group dynamics in sport

    Development of a Cohesion Inventory for Children\u27s Sport Teams

    Get PDF
    The general purpose of this dissertation was to develop an inventory designed to measure cohesion in children’s (ages 9-12) sport teams. To this end, three studies were conducted. In Study 1, children became active agents in the process of test construction. More specifically, children (N = 167) participated in focus groups and completed open-ended questionnaires in order to provide information on their perceptions of cohesion as well as motives for participating, continuing, and ceasing involvement on sport teams. Study 2 involved the use of the information obtained from Study 1 to develop potential items for the questionnaire. In addition, the questionnaires were distributed to child sport participants (N = 298) to determine factorial validity. Finally, the purpose of Study 3 was to establish construct validity for the Child Sport Cohesion Questionnaire (CSCQ) with a heterogeneous sample of children (N = 290). To accomplish this task, four separate tests of validity were assessed: convergent, discriminant, predictive, and factorial. The results from Study 1 indicated that children as young as 9 years can understand the cohesion construct. They are able to identify (a) advantages relating to cohesive teams, (b) disadvantages relating to non-cohesive teams, and (c) methods for increasing cohesion within teams. Finally, in Study 1, children advanced motives for (a) joining, (b) maintaining, and (c) ceasing involvement on sport teams. Study 2 resulted in the development of potential items for the questionnaire. The research team (N = 4) and age appropriate children (N = 8) provided content validity for the advanced items. Furthermore, factorial validity was demonstrated via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). The resulting Child Sport Cohesion Questionnaire containing 16 items (task cohesion = 7, social cohesion = 7, negative spurious items = 2) and a 5-point Likert scale, demonstrated a strong model fit, good internal consistency values, and a moderate inter-factor correlation. The findings from Study 3 revealed that the newly developed CSCQ possessed (a) convergent validity, (b) discriminant validity, and (c) factorial validity. In addition, partial support for predictive validity was established. Overall, the combination of the results from Study 3 provides support for the construct validity of the CSCQ

    Children’s Perceptions of Cohesion

    Get PDF
    The general purpose of the two studies reported here was to examine perceptions of team cohesiveness in children aged 9 to 12 years. In Study 1, focus groups were used to examine individual perceptions of cohesion from the perspective of group integration—the group as a totality. In Study 2, open-ended questionnaires were used to examine individual perceptions of cohesion from the perspective of individual attractions to the group. The results showed that children as young as nine years understand the phenomenon known as cohesion. They can discuss the group as a totality, the characteristics of cohesive and non-cohesive teams, and identify the major factors attracting them to and maintaining their involvement in the group. Also, the ability to clearly distinguish between task and social cohesion is present. The results provide insight into the world of child sport and emphasise the importance of group cohesion, affiliation, and other social constructs in children’s involvement and adherence to sport groups

    Information-theoretic analysis of the directional influence between cellular processes

    Full text link
    Inferring the directionality of interactions between cellular processes is a major challenge in systems biology. Time-lagged correlations allow to discriminate between alternative models, but they still rely on assumed underlying interactions. Here, we use the transfer entropy (TE), an information-theoretic quantity that quantifies the directional influence between fluctuating variables in a model-free way. We present a theoretical approach to compute the transfer entropy, even when the noise has an extrinsic component or in the presence of feedback. We re-analyze the experimental data from Kiviet et al. (2014) where fluctuations in gene expression of metabolic enzymes and growth rate have been measured in single cells of E. coli. We confirm the formerly detected modes between growth and gene expression, while prescribing more stringent conditions on the structure of noise sources. We furthermore point out practical requirements in terms of length of time series and sampling time which must be satisfied in order to infer optimally transfer entropy from times series of fluctuations.Comment: 24 pages, 7 figure

    The magnetization-driven random field Ising model at T=0

    Get PDF
    We study the hysteretic evolution of the random field Ising model (RFIM) at T=0 when the magnetization M is controlled externally and the magnetic field H becomes the output variable. The dynamics is a simple modification of the single-spin-flip dynamics used in the H-driven situation and consists in flipping successively the spins with the largest local field. This allows to perform a detailed comparison between the microscopic trajectories followed by the system with the two protocols. Simulations are performed on random graphs with connectivity z=4 (Bethe lattice) and on the 3-D cubic lattice. The same internal energy U(M)is found with the two protocols when there is no macroscopic avalanche and it does not depend on whether the microscopic states are stable or not. On the Bethe lattice, the energy inside the macroscopic avalanche also coincides with the one that is computed analytically with the H-driven algorithm along the unstable branch of the hysteresis loop. The output field, defined here as dU/dM, exhibits very large fluctuations with the magnetization and is not self-averaging. Relation to the experimental situation is discussed.Comment: 11 pages, 13 figure

    Investigating perceptions of cohesion, performance, and satisfaction in sport officiating groups

    Get PDF
    Since sport officials constitute instrumental groups, their perceptions of, and interactions with, group members likely influence their performance, satisfaction, and retention. This warrants investigation into sport officiating groups. Rationale/Purpose: (1) Examine the relationship between sport officials’ cohesion, satisfaction, and performance; (2) Investigate sport officials’ perceptions of cohesion across sports; and (3) Explore sport officials’ perceptions of group processes. Design/Methodology/Approach: Using a cross-sectional design, participants (N = 228) completed a survey measuring perceptions of cohesion, performance, and satisfaction. Findings: Responses demonstrated consistent positive relationships between cohesion, performance, and satisfaction. Path analysis found that task cohesion predicted performance and satisfaction. Participants rated task cohesion higher than social cohesion, with American football highest and Association football lowest. Practical implications: Officiating organizations can use these results to reconsider assigning practices and develop strategies that improve cohesion, leading to increased performance and retention. Research contribution: Results highlight the need for sustained research to further understand how group processes influence sport officials and their performances. This study is novel as there is a dearth of research on how group dynamics influence sport officials’ performances and retention.</p

    Thriving Together: Conceptual and Methodological Considerations for Examining Thriving in Interdependent Sport

    Get PDF
    Despite conceptual advances and preliminary associations highlighting the benefits of thriving in sport, opportunities for continued research are numerous. Notably, sport-specific research involving thriving has predominantly taken an individual athlete perspective. Interestingly, evidence from the organisational domain suggests that thriving can manifest at a collective level through interdependent team member interactions. Given the potential for thriving to emerge as a higher-level phenomenon in interdependent sport, a critique of thriving at the group-level is advanced. More specifically, we provide a summary of existing individual athlete thriving literature and organisational thriving research at the group-level (Part 1), propose three approaches to conceptualising thriving in interdependent sport (i.e., common, team, and collective thriving) grounded in multilevel research (Part 2), pose guiding questions and key considerations for future exploration (Part 3), and conclude by emphasising the potential value of examining thriving as a higher-level construct for sport researchers and invested partners (Part 4)
    • …
    corecore