925 research outputs found

    Topological inference for EEG and MEG

    Full text link
    Neuroimaging produces data that are continuous in one or more dimensions. This calls for an inference framework that can handle data that approximate functions of space, for example, anatomical images, time--frequency maps and distributed source reconstructions of electromagnetic recordings over time. Statistical parametric mapping (SPM) is the standard framework for whole-brain inference in neuroimaging: SPM uses random field theory to furnish pp-values that are adjusted to control family-wise error or false discovery rates, when making topological inferences over large volumes of space. Random field theory regards data as realizations of a continuous process in one or more dimensions. This contrasts with classical approaches like the Bonferroni correction, which consider images as collections of discrete samples with no continuity properties (i.e., the probabilistic behavior at one point in the image does not depend on other points). Here, we illustrate how random field theory can be applied to data that vary as a function of time, space or frequency. We emphasize how topological inference of this sort is invariant to the geometry of the manifolds on which data are sampled. This is particularly useful in electromagnetic studies that often deal with very smooth data on scalp or cortical meshes. This application illustrates the versatility and simplicity of random field theory and the seminal contributions of Keith Worsley (1951--2009), a key architect of topological inference.Comment: Published in at http://dx.doi.org/10.1214/10-AOAS337 the Annals of Applied Statistics (http://www.imstat.org/aoas/) by the Institute of Mathematical Statistics (http://www.imstat.org

    Emotional representations of space vary as a function of peoples' affect and interoceptive sensibility

    Get PDF
    Most research on people’s representation of space has focused on spatial appraisal and navigation. But there is more to space besides navigation and assessment: people have different emotional experiences at different places, which create emotionally tinged representations of space. Little is known about the emotional representation of space and the factors that shape it. The purpose of this study was to develop a graphic methodology to study the emotional representation of space and some of the environmental features (non-natural vs. natural) and personal features (affective state and interoceptive sensibility) that modulate it. We gave participants blank maps of the region where they lived and asked them to apply shade where they had happy/sad memories, and where they wanted to go after Covid-19 lockdown. Participants also completed self-reports on affective state and interoceptive sensibility. By adapting methods for analyzing neuroimaging data, we examined shaded pixels to quantify where and how strong emotions are represented in space. The results revealed that happy memories were consistently associated with similar spatial locations. Yet, this mapping response varied as a function of participants’ affective state and interoceptive sensibility. Certain regions were associated with happier memories in participants whose affective state was more positive and interoceptive sensibility was higher. The maps of happy memories, desired locations to visit after lockdown, and regions where participants recalled happier memories as a function of positive affect and interoceptive sensibility overlayed significantly with natural environments. These results suggest that people’s emotional representations of their environment are shaped by the naturalness of places, and by their affective state and interoceptive sensibility

    A gene associated with social immunity in the burying beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides.

    Get PDF
    Some group-living species exhibit social immunity, where the immune response of one individual can protect others in the group from infection. In burying beetles, this is part of parental care. Larvae feed on vertebrate carcasses which their parents smear with exudates that inhibit microbial growth. We have sequenced the transcriptome of the burying beetle Nicrophorus vespilloides and identified six genes that encode lysozymes-a type of antimicrobial enzyme that has previously been implicated in social immunity in burying beetles. When females start breeding and producing antimicrobial anal exudates, we found that the expression of one of these genes was increased by approximately 1000 times to become one of the most abundant transcripts in the transcriptome. Females varied considerably in the antimicrobial properties of their anal exudates, and this was strongly correlated with the expression of this lysozyme. We conclude that we have likely identified a gene encoding a key effector molecule in social immunity and that it was recruited during evolution from a function in personal immunity.This work was funded by ERC grant DrosophilaInfection 281668 to F.M.J., NERC grant no. NE/H019731/1 to R.M.K., ERC grant BALDWINIAN_BEETLES 310785 to R.M.K. and a Wolfson Merit Award to R.M.K.This is the final version of the article. It first appeared from Royal Society Publishing via https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.273

    Bend it like Beckham: embodying the motor skills of famous athletes.

    Get PDF
    Observing an action activates the same representations as does the actual performance of the action. Here we show for the first time that the action system can also be activated in the complete absence of action perception. When the participants had to identify the faces of famous athletes, the responses were influenced by their similarity to the motor skills of the athletes. Thus, the motor skills of the viewed athletes were retrieved automatically during person identification and had a direct influence on the action system of the observer. However, our results also indicated that motor behaviours that are implicit characteristics of other people are represented differently from when actions are directly observed. That is, unlike the facilitatory effects reported when actions were seen, the embodiment of the motor behaviour that is not concurrently perceived gave rise to contrast effects where responses similar to the behaviour of the athletes were inhibited

    Studying surface chemistry of mixed conducting perovskite oxide electrodes with synchrotron-based soft X-rays

    Get PDF
    A fundamental understanding of the electrochemical reactions and surface chemistry at the solid–gas interface in situ and operando is critical for electrode materials applied in electrochemical and catalytic applications. Here, the surface reactions and surface composition of a model of mixed ionic and electronic conducting (MIEC) perovskite oxide, (La0.8Sr0.2)0.95Cr0.5Fe0.5O3-δ (LSCrF8255), were investigated in situ using synchrotron-based near-ambient pressure (AP) X-ray photoelectron spectroscopy (XPS) and near-edge X-ray absorption fine-structure spectroscopy (NEXAFS). The measurements were conducted with a surface temperature of 500 °C under 1 mbar of dry oxygen and water vapor, to reflect the implementation of the materials for oxygen reduction/evolution and H2O electrolysis in the applications such as solid oxide fuel cell (SOFC) and electrolyzers. Our direct experimental results demonstrate that, rather than the transition metal (TM) cations, the surface lattice oxygen is the significant redox active species under both dry oxygen and water vapor environments. It was proven that the electron holes formed in dry oxygen have a strong oxygen character. Meanwhile, a relatively higher concentration of surface oxygen vacancies was observed on the sample measured in water vapor. We further showed that in water vapor, the adsorption and dissociation of H2O onto the perovskite surface were through forming hydroxyl groups. In addition, the concentration of Sr surface species was found to increase over time in dry oxygen due to Sr surface segregation, with the presence of oxygen holes on the surface serving as an additional driving force. Comparatively, less Sr contents were observed on the sample in water vapor, which could be due to the volatility of Sr(OH)2. A secondary phase was also observed, which exhibited an enrichment in B-site cations, particularly in Fe and relatively in Cr, and a deficiency in A-site cation, notably in La and relatively in Sr. The findings and methodology of this study allow for the quantification of surface defect chemistry and surface composition evolution, providing crucial understanding and design guidelines in the electrocatalytic activity and durability of electrodes for efficient conversions of energy and fuels

    Linking differences in action perception with differences in action execution.

    Get PDF
    Successful human social interactions depend upon the transmission of verbal and non-verbal signals from one individual to another. Non-verbal social communication is realized through our ability to read and understand information present in other people's actions. It has been proposed that employing the same motor programs, we use to execute an action when observing the same action underlies this action understanding. The main prediction of this framework is that action perception should be strongly correlated with parameters of action execution. Here, we demonstrate that subjects' sensitivity to observed movement speeds is dependent upon how quickly they themselves executed the observed action. This result is consistent with the motor theory of social cognition and suggests that failures in non-verbal social interactions between individuals may in part result from differences in how those individuals move

    Relationship between Activity in Human Primary Motor Cortex during Action Observation and the Mirror Neuron System

    Get PDF
    The attenuation of the beta cortical oscillations during action observation has been interpreted as evidence of a mirror neuron system (MNS) in humans. Here we investigated the modulation of beta cortical oscillations with the viewpoint of an observed action. We asked subjects to observe videos of an actor making a variety of arm movements. We show that when subjects were observing arm movements there was a significant modulation of beta oscillations overlying left and right sensorimotor cortices. This pattern of attenuation was driven by the side of the screen on which the observed movement occurred and not by the hand that was observed moving. These results are discussed in terms of the firing patterns of mirror neurons in F5 which have been reported to have similar properties

    Insight into structure: function relationships in a molecular spin-crossover crystal, from a related weakly cooperative compound

    Get PDF
    This is a repository copy of Insight into structure: function relationships in a molecular spin-crossover crystal, from a related weakly cooperative compound. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/83008/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Elhaïk, J, Kilner, C and Halcrow, MA (2014) Insight into structure: function relationships in a molecular spin-crossover crystal, from a related weakly cooperative compound. European Journal of Inorganic Chemistry, 2014 (26). 4250 -4253. ISSN 14344250 -4253. ISSN -1948 https://doi.org/10.1002/ejic.201402623 [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Reuse Unless indicated otherwise, fulltext items are protected by copyright with all rights reserved. The copyright exception in section 29 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 allows the making of a single copy solely for the purpose of non-commercial research or private study within the limits of fair dealing. The publisher or other rights-holder may allow further reproduction and re-use of this version -refer to the White Rose Research Online record for this item. Where records identify the publisher as the copyright holder, users can verify any specific terms of use on the publisher's website. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. Insight into Compound Jérôme Elhaïk, [a] Colin A. Kilner, [a] and Malcolm A. Halcrow* [a] Abstract: The ClO4 − salt of [FeL2] 2+ (L = 2,6-bis(3-methylpyrazol-1-yl)pyridine) undergoes very gradual thermal spin-crossover centered just below room temperature. In contrast, the BF4 − salt of the same complex exhibits an abrupt and structured spin-transition at lower temperature, with a complicated structural chemistry. The difference can be attributed to a much larger change in molecular structure between the spin states of the complex in the more cooperative BF4 − salt, leading to an increased kinetic barrier for their interconversion. Consistent with that suggestion, the high-spin and low-spin structures of weakly cooperative [FeL2][ClO4]2 are almost superimposable. The continuing interest in thermally and optically switchable spin-crossover (SCO) materials [9] Its thermal spin-transition takes place in two steps, via a re-entrant symmetry-breaking transition to an intermediate crystal phase, with a tripled unit cell containing a mixture of high-spin and low-spin sites. The first of these steps occurs abruptly with hysteresis, but at a temperature that varies according to the water content of the sample (x). In contrast the second step is kinetically slow, and is only achieved when the sample is poised at 100 K for 1.5 hrs. [10] Its excited spin-state trapping (LIESST [11] ) behavior is also unique, in that its thermodynamic high low spin transition and kinetically controlled high low spin-state relaxation exhibit different profiles and are effectively decoupled from each other. [12] Although unexceptional in itself, 1[ClO4]2 provides useful insight into the structural origin of the unusual behavior of the BF4 − salt by providing a rare comparison between strongly and weakly cooperative spin-crossover materials based on the same complex molecule. At 300 K, MT for 1[ClO4]2 is 2.4 cm 3 mol -1 K, lower than expected for a high-spin iron(II) complex with this ligand type (3.4-3.6 cm 3 mol -1 K)

    Facial Emotion Recognition and Expression in Parkinson's Disease: An Emotional Mirror Mechanism?

    Get PDF
    BACKGROUND AND AIM: Parkinson's disease (PD) patients have impairment of facial expressivity (hypomimia) and difficulties in interpreting the emotional facial expressions produced by others, especially for aversive emotions. We aimed to evaluate the ability to produce facial emotional expressions and to recognize facial emotional expressions produced by others in a group of PD patients and a group of healthy participants in order to explore the relationship between these two abilities and any differences between the two groups of participants. METHODS: Twenty non-demented, non-depressed PD patients and twenty healthy participants (HC) matched for demographic characteristics were studied. The ability of recognizing emotional facial expressions was assessed with the Ekman 60-faces test (Emotion recognition task). Participants were video-recorded while posing facial expressions of 6 primary emotions (happiness, sadness, surprise, disgust, fear and anger). The most expressive pictures for each emotion were derived from the videos. Ten healthy raters were asked to look at the pictures displayed on a computer-screen in pseudo-random fashion and to identify the emotional label in a six-forced-choice response format (Emotion expressivity task). Reaction time (RT) and accuracy of responses were recorded. At the end of each trial the participant was asked to rate his/her confidence in his/her perceived accuracy of response. RESULTS: For emotion recognition, PD reported lower score than HC for Ekman total score (p<0.001), and for single emotions sub-scores happiness, fear, anger, sadness (p<0.01) and surprise (p = 0.02). In the facial emotion expressivity task, PD and HC significantly differed in the total score (p = 0.05) and in the sub-scores for happiness, sadness, anger (all p<0.001). RT and the level of confidence showed significant differences between PD and HC for the same emotions. There was a significant positive correlation between the emotion facial recognition and expressivity in both groups; the correlation was even stronger when ranking emotions from the best recognized to the worst (R = 0.75, p = 0.004). CONCLUSIONS: PD patients showed difficulties in recognizing emotional facial expressions produced by others and in posing facial emotional expressions compared to healthy subjects. The linear correlation between recognition and expression in both experimental groups suggests that the two mechanisms share a common system, which could be deteriorated in patients with PD. These results open new clinical and rehabilitation perspectives

    Why I tense up when you watch me: inferior parietal cortex mediates an audience’s influence on motor performance

    Get PDF
    The presence of an evaluative audience can alter skilled motor performance through changes in force output. To investigate how this is mediated within the brain, we emulated real-time social monitoring of participants’ performance of a fine grip task during functional magnetic resonance neuroimaging. We observed an increase in force output during social evaluation that was accompanied by focal reductions in activity within bilateral inferior parietal cortex. Moreover, deactivation of the left inferior parietal cortex predicted both inter- and intra-individual differences in socially-induced change in grip force. Social evaluation also enhanced activation within the posterior superior temporal sulcus, which conveys visual information about others’ actions to the inferior parietal cortex. Interestingly, functional connectivity between these two regions was attenuated by social evaluation. Our data suggest that social evaluation can vary force output through the altered engagement of inferior parietal cortex; a region implicated in sensorimotor integration necessary for object manipulation, and a component of the action-observation network which integrates and facilitates performance of observed actions. Social-evaluative situations may induce high-level representational incoherence between one’s own intentioned action and the perceived intention of others which, by uncoupling the dynamics of sensorimotor facilitation, could ultimately perturbe motor output
    corecore