23 research outputs found

    Specific Gestalt principles cannot explain (un)crowding

    Get PDF
    The standard physiological model has serious problems accounting for many aspects of vision, particularly when stimulus configurations become slightly more complex than the ones classically used, e.g., configurations of Gabors rather than only one or a few Gabors. For example, as shown in many publications, crowding cannot be explained with most models crafted in the spirit of the physiological approach. In crowding, a target is neighbored by flanking elements, which impair target discrimination. However, when more flankers are added, performance can improve for certain flanker configurations (uncrowding), which cannot be explained by classic models. As was shown, aspects of perceptual organization play a crucial role in uncrowding. For this reason, we tested here whether known principles of perceptual organization can explain crowding and uncrowding. The answer is negative. As shown with subjective tests, whereas grouping is indeed key in uncrowding, the four Gestalt principles examined here did not provide a clear explanation to this effect, as variability in performance was found between and within categories of configurations. We discuss the philosophical foundations of both the physiological and the classic Gestalt approaches and sketch a way to a happy marriage between the two

    The dynamics of statistical learning in visual search and its interaction with salience processing: an EEG study

    Get PDF
    Visual attention can be guided by statistical regularities in the environment, that people implicitly learn from past experiences (statistical learning, SL). Moreover, a perceptually salient element can automatically capture attention, gaining processing priority through a bottom-up attentional control mechanism. The aim of our study was to investigate the dynamics of SL and if it shapes attentional target selection additively with salience processing, or whether these mechanisms interact, e.g. one gates the other. In a visual search task, we therefore manipulated target frequency (high vs. low) across locations while, in some trials, the target was salient in terms of colour. Additionally, halfway through the experiment, the high-frequency location changed to the opposite hemifield. EEG activity was simultaneously recorded, with a specific interest in two markers related to target selection and post-selection processing, respectively: N2pc and SPCN. Our results revealed that both SL and saliency significantly enhanced behavioural performance, but also interacted with each other, with an attenuated saliency effect at the high-frequency target location, and a smaller SL effect for salient targets. Concerning processing dynamics, the benefit of salience processing was more evident during the early stage of target selection and processing, as indexed by a larger N2pc and early-SPCN, whereas SL modulated the underlying neural activity particularly later on, as revealed by larger late-SPCN. Furthermore, we showed that SL was rapidly acquired and adjusted when the spatial imbalance changed. Overall, our findings suggest that SL is flexible to changes and, combined with salience processing, jointly contributes to establishing attentional priority

    Integrated effects of top-down attention and statistical learning during visual search: an EEG study

    Get PDF
    The present study aims to investigate how the competition between visual elements is solved by top-down and/or statistical learning (SL) attentional control (AC) mechanisms when active together. We hypothesized that the "winner" element that will undergo further processing is selected either by one AC mechanism that prevails over the other, or by the joint activity of both mechanisms. To test these hypotheses, we conducted a visual search experiment that combined an endogenous cueing protocol (valid vs. neutral cue) and an imbalance of target frequency distribution across locations (high- vs. low-frequency location). The unique and combined effects of top-down control and SL mechanisms were measured on behaviour and amplitudes of three evoked-response potential (ERP) components (i.e., N2pc, P1, CNV) related to attentional processing. Our behavioural results showed better performance for validly cued targets and for targets in the high-frequency location. The two factors were found to interact, so that SL effects emerged only in the absence of top-down guidance. Whereas the CNV and P1 only displayed a main effect of cueing, for the N2pc we observed an interaction between cueing and SL, revealing a cueing effect for targets in the low-frequency condition, but not in the high-frequency condition. Thus, our data support the view that top-down control and SL work in a conjoint, integrated manner during target selection. In particular, SL mechanisms are reduced or even absent when a fully reliable top-down guidance of attention is at play

    The competition between grouping cues can be resolved under inattention_sup

    No full text
    supplementary - individual means and RT delta plots for Rashal & Kimchi (AP&P

    Contrast dissimilarity effects on crowding are not simply another case of target saliency

    No full text

    Depth from blur and grouping under inattention

    No full text
    Previous studies provided evidence in support of attention operating in three-dimensional space, and the iterative and multistage nature of organizational processes in relation to attention and depth. We investigated depth perception and attentional demands in grouping organizations that contain blur as a depth cue. Contrary to previous studies, in our displays, no depth from occlusion could be implied from a shared border between groups or surfaces. To evaluate depth perception, subjective reports were collected where participants indicated which elements, blurry or sharp, they perceived as closer. To examine whether depth perception from blur can alleviate attentional demands, we used an inattention paradigm. We presented displays of grouping organizations by collinearity or color similarity that were previously found to require attention and added blur to the figure or the background elements to generate depth perception. In addition, we presented similar displays containing grouping by blur similarity as a single cue. We hypothesized that adding blur would facilitate the segmentation of element groups due to their perceived depth, which might lead to a diminished demand for attention. Our results confirmed that blur led to depth perception, and that sharp elements were perceived as closer more frequently than blurry elements. Thus, these results provide novel evidence for depth from blur in grouping where no inference of occlusion can be derived from a border. However, although the results suggest that blur information was processed under inattention, little evidence was found for decreased attentional demands for grouping processes in the presence of blur.status: publishe
    corecore