7 research outputs found

    How a Lateralized Brain Supports Symmetrical Bimanual Tasks

    Get PDF
    A large repertoire of natural object manipulation tasks require precisely coupled symmetrical opposing forces by both hands on a single object. We asked how the lateralized brain handles this basic problem of spatial and temporal coordination. We show that the brain consistently appoints one of the hands as prime actor while the other assists, but the choice of acting hand is flexible. When study participants control a cursor by manipulating a tool held freely between the hands, the left hand becomes prime actor if the cursor moves directionally with the left-hand forces, whereas the right hand primarily acts if it moves with the opposing right-hand forces. In neurophysiological (electromyography, transcranial magnetic brain stimulation) and functional magnetic resonance brain imaging experiments we demonstrate that changes in hand assignment parallels a midline shift of lateralized activity in distal hand muscles, corticospinal pathways, and primary sensorimotor and cerebellar cortical areas. We conclude that the two hands can readily exchange roles as dominant actor in bimanual tasks. Spatial relationships between hand forces and goal motions determine hand assignments rather than habitual handedness. Finally, flexible role assignment of the hands is manifest at multiple levels of the motor system, from cortical regions all the way down to particular muscles

    To select one hand while using both : neural mechanisms supporting flexible hand dominance in bimanual object manipulation

    No full text
    In daily activities, the brain regularly assigns different roles to the hands dependingon task and context. Yet, little is known about the underlying neural processes. Thiscertainly applies to how the brain, where each hemisphere primarily controls onehand, manages the between-hand coordination required in bimanual objectmanipulation. By using behavioral, neurophysiological and functional magneticresonance imaging techniques, the present thesis examines neural mechanisms thatsupport hand coordination during tasks where the two hands apply spatiotemporallycoupled but opposing forces for goal attainment, e.g., as when removing the cap froma bottle. Although the two hands seem to operate symmetrically in such tasks, Study Ishowed that one hand primarily acts while the other assists. Moreover, this roledifferentiation was found to be flexible with the brain appointing either hand asprime actor depending on the spatial congruency between hand forces and desiredmovement consequences. Accordingly, when we remove a cap from a bottle, the handthat grasps the cap, be it left or right depending on overall task constraints, isappointed as prime actor because the twist forces it generates are aligned with thegoal to remove the cap, while the other hand, holding the bottle, applies stabilizingforces in the opposite direction. Changes in hand assignments are caused by amidline shift of lateralized activity throughout the motor system, from distal handmuscles to corticospinal pathways and primary sensorimotor and cerebellar corticalareas (Study I). Although the bimanual actions examined involved both within- andbetween-hand coordination, Study II failed to reveal additional brain activity duringbimanual as compared to matching unimanual actions, except for the primarysensorimotor areas where subpopulations of neurons were preferentially engagedduring either bimanual or unimanual actions. Thus, dedicated neurons in the motorcortices might support critical bimanual coordinative operations. While imagingresults indicated that a mainly left-lateralized parietal-premotor network managedthe task irrespective of prime actor, premotor areas presumably established handassignment by allocating the lead either to the left or the right primary sensorimotorareas (Study I and II). Regarding the process of prime actor selection and hence thecontrol of these premotor networks, imaging results indicate a transitory involvementof prefrontal cortical areas (Study III). The detected areas belong to a networkconsidered critical for cognitive operations such as judgment and decision-making,and for evaluation of utility of actions, including conflict detection. The implicitselection of prime actor during bimanual tasks thus seems to be supported by corticalareas traditionally associated primarily with complex cognitive challenges

    Influence of Mapping Rule on Tool Movements during Performance with Left-Hand and Right-Hand Maps

    No full text
    <p>Superimposed time traces of longitudinal force and lateral tool movement (upper panels) and of twist force and rotational tool movement (lower panels) from a single participant during the last 20 s of target chasing in the first experiment; <i>r<sub>LO</sub></i> and <i>r<sub>TW</sub></i> indicate hand-asymmetry indices for longitudinal and twist forces. Bottom trace represents instances of target hits (spikes). For the last 20 s of runs by all participants and mapping rules, the slope coefficients of the linear regressions indicated that the tool moved 0.84 (0.28–1.23) mm/N longitudinal force and rotated 0.84 (0.36–1.42) °/N twist force (median and 25th–75th percentile). The corresponding values for the 30-s periods of target chasing for which fMRI data were analyzed (see <a href="http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040158#pbio-0040158-g005" target="_blank">Figure 5</a>A and <a href="http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040158#pbio-0040158-g005" target="_blank">5</a>B), where the wrists of the participants were strapped, were 0.28 (0.19–0.68) mm/N and 0.63 (0.42–1.04) °/N. </p

    The Bimanual Target-Chasing Task and Flexible Role Assignment of the Hands

    No full text
    <div><p>(A) Mappings between applied forces and cursor movements (top graph). With the left-hand map, the cursor moved horizontally in the direction of the longitudinal force applied by the left hand (solid purple arrows). A counter-clockwise twist force applied between the handles (as if unscrewing the lid of a jar) moved the cursor upward (solid green arrows). With the right-hand map the cursor moved in the opposite directions and thus in the direction of the forces of the right hand.</p> <p>(B and C) Performance under each mapping rule shown for a complete first session with 602 hits and from the last 100 hits of a second session. Superimposed thin lines show hit time and path index for each participant as a function of target number (data median-filtered over a ± 10-s period around each hit). Solid curve give medians across participants. Inserts in (C) exemplify cursor trajectories with a median path index of 4.6 and 1.4 across 10 target transitions. The targets, distributed about uniformly over the screen, were located 5.1 ± 2.1° (mean ± 1 SD) visual angle from its center, which corresponded to 2.2 ± 0.3 N force applied tangentially to the surfaces of the handles.</p> <p>(D and E) Hand-asymmetry indices computed for a sliding ± 10-s time window. Horizontal lines give the upper and lower 95% confidence limit of the index, postulating that hand selection would have occurred randomly. A significant positive and negative index indicates left and right-hand primarily acting, respectively.</p></div

    Premotor Cortical Areas with Increased BOLD Responses with Both the Left- and the Right-Hand Map Overlaid on Coronal Slices of the MNI T1-Weighted Brain Template

    No full text
    <p>For the left hemisphere (L), significant activations ( <i>p</i> < 0.01, FWE-corrected) occurred in one cluster (443 voxels) with two maxima in precentral gyrus (#2 and #4; BA 6), in one single-peak cluster (153 voxels) in superior frontal gyrus (#3; BA 6), in one cluster (281 voxels) with two maxima in medial frontal gyrus (#5 and #6; BA 6), and in one small single-peak cluster (29 voxels) in left inferior frontal gyrus (#1; BA 44). In the right hemisphere (R), there was one cluster (303 voxels) with three maxima, one of which was located in the precentral gyrus outside the cluster delineated by left-hand-map > right-hand-map contrast (#7; BA 6). Solid black lines in the left and right hemisphere outline the clusters identified with the right-hand-map > left-hand-map and left-hand-map > right-hand-map contrasts, respectively (see <a href="http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pbio.0040158#pbio-0040158-g004" target="_blank">Figure 4</a>C). Histograms give percent BOLD signal change relative to mean of session for the local maxima of identified clusters. Red and blue columns refer to left- and right-hand maps, respectively. Column height gives data averaged across participants and error bar ± 1SEM ( <i>n</i> = 16). Coordinates (X, Y, Z in MNI stereotaxic space) and <i>t</i><sub>(30)</sub> values for the maxima are presented below each histogram. </p
    corecore