301 research outputs found

    Cardio-metabolic risk factors and cortical thickness in a neurologically healthy male population: results from the psychological, social and biological determinants of ill health (pSoBid) study

    Get PDF
    <p>Introduction: Cardio-metabolic risk factors have been associated with poor physical and mental health. Epidemiological studies have shown peripheral risk markers to be associated with poor cognitive functioning in normal healthy population and in disease. The aim of the study was to explore the relationship between cardio-metabolic risk factors and cortical thickness in a neurologically healthy middle aged population-based sample.</p> <p>Methods: T1-weighted MRI was used to create models of the cortex for calculation of regional cortical thickness in 40 adult males (average ageย =ย 50.96ย years), selected from the PSOBID study. The relationship between cardio-vascular risk markers and cortical thickness across the whole brain, were examined using the general linear models. The relationship with various covariates of interest was explored.</p> <p>Results: Lipid fractions with greater triglyceride content (TAG, VLDL and LDL) were associated with greater cortical thickness pertaining to a number of regions in the brain. Greater C reactive protein (CRP) and Intercellular adhesion molecule (ICAM-1) levels were associated with cortical thinning pertaining to perisylvian regions in the left hemisphere. Smoking status and education status were significant covariates in the model.</p> <p>Conclusions: This exploratory study adds to a small body of existing literature increasingly showing a relationship between cardio-metabolic risk markers and regional cortical thickness involving a number of regions in the brain in a neurologically normal middle aged sample. A focused investigation of factors determining the inter-individual variations in regional cortical thickness in the adult brain could provide further clarity in our understanding of the relationship between cardio-metabolic factors and cortical structures.</p&gt

    the wearable devices opportunity

    Get PDF
    ํ•™์œ„๋…ผ๋ฌธ(๋ฐ•์‚ฌ) -- ์„œ์šธ๋Œ€ํ•™๊ต๋Œ€ํ•™์› : ์˜๊ณผ๋Œ€ํ•™ ์˜ํ•™๊ณผ, 2022. 8. ๊น€๊ธฐ์›….๋ฐฐ๊ฒฝ ๋ฐ ๋ชฉ์ : ์น˜๋งค๋กœ ์ธํ•œ ๊ณต๊ณต๋ณด๊ฑด ๋ถ€๋‹ด์ด ๊ฐ€์ค‘๋จ์—๋„ ๋งŒ์กฑ์Šค๋Ÿฌ์šด ์น˜๋ฃŒ๋ฒ•์€ ๋ถ€์žฌํ•œ ํ˜„ ์ƒํ™ฉ์€ ์น˜๋งค ๋ฐœ๋ณ‘์„ ์˜ˆ๋ฐฉํ•˜๊ฑฐ๋‚˜ ์ง„ํ–‰์„ ์ง€์—ฐ์‹œํ‚ค๊ธฐ ์œ„ํ•ด ์ธ์ง€์ €ํ•˜ ๋˜๋Š” ์น˜๋งค ์œ„ํ—˜์ด ์žˆ๋Š” ์‚ฌ๋žŒ๋“ค์„ ์กฐ๊ธฐ์— ์‹๋ณ„ํ•ด์•ผ ํ•  ํ•„์š”๋ฅผ ๋”์šฑ ๋ถ€๊ฐ์‹œํ‚จ๋‹ค. ์ตœ๊ทผ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๋“ค์€ ๋ณดํ–‰ ์‹œ ํ•œ๋ฐœ-ํ•œ๋ฐœ ์‚ฌ์ด ๋ณดํ–‰์ธ์ž๋“ค์˜ ๋ณ€๋™์„ฑ์„ ์˜๋ฏธํ•˜๋Š” ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ์ธ์ง€ ์ €ํ•˜, ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€์žฅ์•  ๋ฐ ์น˜๋งค์˜ ์œ„ํ—˜๊ณผ ๋ฐ€์ ‘ํ•˜๊ฒŒ ๊ด€๋ จ๋˜์–ด ์žˆ๋‹ค๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์„ ๋ณด์˜€๋‹ค. ํŠนํžˆ ์›จ์–ด๋Ÿฌ๋ธ” ์„ผ์„œ๋ฅผ ํ†ตํ•ด ์–ป์€ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์€ ๊ฐ๋…์ด ์—†๋Š” ์ž์—ฐ์Šค๋Ÿฌ์šด ํ™˜๊ฒฝ์—์„œ ๋” ์˜ค๋žœ ๊ธฐ๊ฐ„ ๋™์•ˆ ์ธก์ •๊ฐ’์„ ๋‚ฎ์€ ๋น„์šฉ์œผ๋กœ ์–ป์„ ์ˆ˜ ์žˆ๋‹ค๋Š” ์‹ค์šฉ์ ์ธ ์ด์ ์œผ๋กœ ์ธํ•ด ์ธ์ง€์ €ํ•˜์˜ ์œ„ํ—˜์„ ์˜ˆ์ธกํ•˜๋Š” ์œ ๋งํ•œ ๋””์ง€ํ„ธ ๋ฐ”์ด์˜ค๋งˆ์ปค๋กœ ํ™œ์šฉ๋  ์ˆ˜ ์žˆ๋‹ค. ๋ณธ ๋…ผ๋ฌธ์—์„œ๋Š”, ์‹ ์ฒด์— ๋ถ€์ฐฉํ•œ ๋‹จ์ผ ์‚ผ์ถ•๊ฐ€์†๊ณ„๋กœ ์ธก์ •๋œ ๋ณดํ–‰ ๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ๋ฏธ๋ž˜์˜ ์ธ์ง€์ €ํ•˜ ์œ„ํ—˜์„ ์˜ˆ์ธกํ•˜๋Š” ๋””์ง€ํ„ธ ๋ฐ”์ด์˜ค๋งˆ์ปค๋กœ ์‚ฌ์šฉ๋  ์ˆ˜ ์žˆ์„์ง€์— ๋Œ€ํ•ด ์—ฐ๊ตฌํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ์ฒซ ๋ฒˆ์งธ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ์—์„œ๋Š” ์‹ ์ฒด ๋ถ€์ฐฉ ์‚ผ์ถ•๊ฐ€์†๊ณ„๋กœ ์–ป์€ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ์ •์ƒ์ธ์ง€๋ฅผ ๊ฐ€์ง„ ๋…ธ์ธ์—์„œ ๋ฏธ๋ž˜ ์ธ์ง€์ €ํ•˜์˜ ์œ„ํ—˜์„ ์˜ˆ์ธกํ•  ์ˆ˜ ์žˆ๋Š”์ง€๋ฅผ ์กฐ์‚ฌํ–ˆ๋‹ค. ๋‘ ๋ฒˆ์งธ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ์—์„œ๋Š” ๋” ํฐ ํ‘œ๋ณธ ํฌ๊ธฐ์™€ ๋” ๋„“์€ ๋ฒ”์œ„์˜ ์ธ์ง€ ๊ธฐ๋Šฅ์„ ๊ฐ€์ง„ ๋น„์น˜๋งค ๋…ธ์ธ์„ ๋Œ€์ƒ์œผ๋กœ, ๋””์ง€ํ„ธ ๋ฐ”์ด์˜ค๋งˆ์ปค๋กœ์„œ ๋ณดํ–‰ ๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์˜ ๊ฐ€๋Šฅ์„ฑ์„ ์ด๋ก ์ ์œผ๋กœ ๋’ท๋ฐ›์นจํ•  ์ˆ˜ ์žˆ๋Š” ์‹ ๊ฒฝ๊ธฐ์งˆ์— ๋Œ€ํ•ด ์กฐ์‚ฌํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ๋˜ํ•œ, ๋†’์€ ๋ณดํ–‰ ๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ์ธ์ง€ ๊ธฐ๋Šฅ ๋ฐ ๊ธฐ์–ต ๊ธฐ๋Šฅ์— ๊ด€๋ จ๋œ ๊ฒƒ์œผ๋กœ ๋ฐํ˜€์ง„ ๋‡Œ ์˜์—ญ์—์„œ์˜ ์–‡์•„์ง„ ๋Œ€๋‡Œ ํ”ผ์งˆ ๋‘๊ป˜์™€ ๊ด€๋ จ๋˜์–ด ์žˆ์„ ๊ฒƒ์ด๋ฉฐ, ๊ทธ ์˜์—ญ์ด ๋ณดํ–‰-์ธ์ง€ ์‚ฌ์ด์˜ ์—ฐ๊ด€์„ฑ์„ ์„ค๋ช…ํ•˜๋Š” ๊ณต์œ  ์‹ ๊ฒฝ ๊ธฐ์งˆ์— ํ•ด๋‹นํ•  ๊ฒƒ์ด๋ผ๋Š” ๊ฐ€์„ค์„ ๊ฒ€์ฆํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ๋ฐฉ๋ฒ•: ์—ฐ๊ตฌ I์—์„œ ์šฐ๋ฆฌ๋Š” ๋‡Œํ—ˆํ˜ˆ์ด๋‚˜ ํŒŒํ‚จ์Šจ๋ณ‘์ด ์—†์œผ๋ฉด์„œ, ์ง€์—ญ์‚ฌํšŒ์— ๊ฑฐ์ฃผํ•˜๋Š”, ์ธ์ง€์ ์œผ๋กœ ์ •์ƒ์ธ ๋…ธ์ธ 91๋ช…์„ ๋Œ€์ƒ์œผ๋กœ 4๋…„ ์ „ํ–ฅ์  ์ฝ”ํ˜ธํŠธ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๋ฅผ ์ˆ˜ํ–‰ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ์ฒด์ค‘์‹ฌ์— ๋ถ€์ฐฉํ•œ ์‚ผ์ถ•๊ฐ€์†๊ณ„๋ฅผ ์ด์šฉํ•˜์—ฌ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์„ ์ธก์ •ํ•˜์˜€๊ณ , ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€์žฅ์• ์— ๊ด€ํ•œ ๊ตญ์ œ ์›Œํ‚น ๊ทธ๋ฃน์˜ ์ง„๋‹จ๊ธฐ์ค€์— ๋”ฐ๋ผ ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€ ์žฅ์• ๋ฅผ ์ง„๋‹จํ–ˆ๋‹ค. ์šฐ๋ฆฌ๋Š” ๋ณดํ–‰ ๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์˜ ํฌ๊ธฐ์— ๋”ฐ๋ผ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๋Œ€์ƒ์ž๋ฅผ ์‚ผ๋ถ„์œ„์ˆ˜๋กœ ๋ถ„๋ฅ˜ํ•˜์—ฌ, ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ๊ฐ€์žฅ ํฐ ์ผ๋ถ„์œ„ ๊ทธ๋ฃน๊ณผ ๋‚˜๋จธ์ง€ ๊ทธ๋ฃน์„ ๊ด€์ฐฐํ•˜๋ฉฐ 4๋…„ ๋™์•ˆ์˜ ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€์žฅ์• ์˜ ๋ฐœ๋ณ‘์„ ์ถ”์ ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ๊ทธ๋ฃน๊ฐ„ ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€์žฅ์•  ๋ฐœ๋ณ‘ ์œ„ํ—˜ ๋น„๊ต๋Š” Log-rank test์™€ Kaplan-Meier ๋ถ„์„์„ ํ†ตํ•ด ์ˆ˜ํ–‰ํ–ˆ๋‹ค. ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€์žฅ์•  ๋ฐœ๋ณ‘ ์œ„ํ—˜๋น„(Hazard Ratio, HR)๋Š” ์—ฐ๋ น, ์„ฑ๋ณ„, ๊ต์œก์ˆ˜์ค€, ๋ˆ„์ ์งˆ๋ณ‘ํ‰๊ฐ€์ฒ™๋„ ์ ์ˆ˜, GDS ์ ์ˆ˜, ์•„ํฌ์ง€๋‹จ๋ฐฑ E ฮต4 ๋Œ€๋ฆฝ์œ ์ „์ž ์œ ๋ฌด๋ฅผ ๋ณด์ •ํ•œ ์ฝ•์Šค ๋น„๋ก€์œ„ํ—˜ ํšŒ๊ท€ ๋ถ„์„์„ ์‚ฌ์šฉํ•˜์—ฌ ์ถ”์ •ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ์—ฐ๊ตฌ II์—์„œ ์šฐ๋ฆฌ๋Š” 207๋ช…์˜ ์น˜๋งค๊ฐ€ ์—†๋Š” ๋…ธ์ธ์„ ๋Œ€์ƒ์œผ๋กœ, ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ๊ณผ ์—ฐ๊ด€๋œ ๋‡Œ ํ”ผ์งˆ ๋ฐ ํ”ผ์งˆ ํ•˜ ์‹ ๊ฒฝ ๊ตฌ์กฐ, ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ-์ธ์ง€๊ธฐ๋Šฅ์˜ ๊ณต์œ ์‹ ๊ฒฝ๊ธฐ์งˆ์„ ํšก๋‹จ์ ์œผ๋กœ ์—ฐ๊ตฌํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ์ž๊ธฐ๊ณต๋ช…์˜์ƒ์—์„œ ๋‡Œ ํ”ผ์งˆ์˜ ๋‘๊ป˜์™€ ํ”ผ์งˆ ํ•˜ ๊ตฌ์กฐ๋ฌผ ๋ถ€ํ”ผ๋ฅผ ๊ตฌํ•˜์—ฌ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ, ์ธ์ง€๊ธฐ๋Šฅ, ํ”ผ์งˆ ๋‘๊ป˜์™€ ํ”ผ์งˆ ํ•˜ ๊ตฌ์กฐ๋ฌผ ๋ถ€ํ”ผ์™€์˜ ์—ฐ๊ด€์„ฑ์„ ๊ฐ๊ฐ ์กฐ์‚ฌํ–ˆ๋‹ค. ๋˜ํ•œ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ๊ณผ ์ธ์ง€๊ธฐ๋Šฅ ์–‘์ชฝ์— ๋ชจ๋‘ ์œ ์˜ํ•œ ์—ฐ๊ด€์„ฑ์„ ๋ณด์ด๋Š” ๋‡Œ์˜์—ญ์˜ ํ”ผ์งˆ ๋‘๊ป˜ ๋˜๋Š” ํ”ผ์งˆ ํ•˜ ๊ตฌ์กฐ๋ฌผ ๋ถ€ํ”ผ๊ฐ€ ์‹ค์ œ๋กœ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ๊ณผ ์ธ์ง€๊ธฐ๋Šฅ ๊ด€๊ณ„์— ๋ฏธ์น˜๋Š” ๋งค๊ฐœํšจ๊ณผ๋ฅผ ๋ถ„์„ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ๊ฒฐ๊ณผ: ์—ฐ๊ตฌ I์—์„œ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ์ผ๋ถ„์œ„์— ์†ํ•˜๋Š” ๋…ธ์ธ๋“ค์€ ๋‚˜๋จธ์ง€ ๋…ธ์ธ๋“ค์— ๋น„ํ•ด์„œ 4๋…„ ๊ฐ„ ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€์žฅ์•  ๋ฐœ๋ณ‘ ์œ„ํ—˜์ด ์•ฝ 12๋ฐฐ ๋” ๋†’์•˜๋‹ค. (HR = 11.97, 95% CI = 1.29โ€“111.37). ๊ทธ๋Ÿฌ๋‚˜ ๋Š๋ฆฐ ๋ณดํ–‰ ์†๋„๋ฅผ ๊ฐ€์ง„ ๋…ธ์ธ๋“ค์€ ๋‚˜๋จธ์ง€ ๋…ธ์ธ๋“ค๊ณผ ๋น„์Šทํ•œ ๊ฒฝ๋„์ธ์ง€์žฅ์•  ๋ฐœ๋ณ‘์œ„ํ—˜์„ ๋ณด์˜€๋‹ค. (HR = 5.04, 95% CI = 0.53โ€“48.18). ์šฐ๋ฆฌ๋Š” ๋˜ํ•œ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ๋ฏธ๋ž˜ ์ธ์ง€์ €ํ•˜๋ฅผ ์˜ˆ์ธกํ•˜๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์—๋Š” ์„ฑ๋ณ„์— ๋”ฐ๋ฅธ ์ฐจ์ด๊ฐ€ ์œ ์˜ํ•˜์ง€ ์•Š๋‹ค๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์„ ๋ฐํ˜”๋‹ค. ์—ฐ๊ตฌ ๋Œ€์ƒ์ž๋“ค์„ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์˜ ํฌ๊ธฐ๋กœ ์‚ผ๋ถ„์œ„ํ™” ํ•˜๋Š” ๊ณผ์ •์—์„œ์˜ ์—ญ์น˜ํšจ๊ณผ (threshold effect) ์œ ๋ฌด๋ฅผ ์•Œ์•„๋ณด๊ธฐ ์œ„ํ•ด, ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ๊ณผ ๋ณดํ–‰์†๋„๋ฅผ ์—ฐ์†๋ณ€์ˆ˜๋กœ ๋‘๊ณ  ๋ถ„์„ํ•˜์˜€์„ ๋•Œ์—๋„ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด 10% ์ฆ๊ฐ€ํ•  ๋•Œ๋งˆ๋‹ค ์ธ์ง€์ €ํ•˜์˜ ์œ„ํ—˜์ด 1.5๋ฐฐ ์ฆ๊ฐ€ํ•˜๋Š” ๋ฐ˜๋ฉด ๋ณดํ–‰์†๋„์˜ ๋ณ€ํ™”์— ๋”ฐ๋ผ ์ธ์ง€๊ฐํ‡ด ์œ„ํ—˜์˜ ์œ ์˜ํ•œ ๋ณ€ํ™”๋Š” ์—†์—ˆ๋‹ค. ์—ฐ๊ตฌ II์—์„œ ๋†’์€ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์€ ๋‚ฎ์€ ์ธ์ง€๊ธฐ๋Šฅ๊ณผ ๊ด€๋ จ์ด ์žˆ์—ˆ๋‹ค. ์šฐ๋ฆฌ๋Š” ๋†’์€ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์ด ๊ด‘๋ฒ”์œ„ํ•œ ์˜์—ญ์—์„œ ๋Œ€๋‡Œํ”ผ์งˆ ๋‘๊ป˜ ๊ฐ์†Œ์™€ ์—ฐ๊ด€์ด ์žˆ๋‹ค๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์„ ํ™•์ธํ–ˆ๋‹ค. ๋ฐ˜๋ฉด, ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์€ ํ”ผ์งˆ ํ•˜ ๊ตฌ์กฐ๋ฌผ์˜ ๋ถ€ํ”ผ์™€๋Š” ์œ ์˜ํ•œ ์—ฐ๊ด€์„ฑ์„ ๋ณด์ด์ง€ ์•Š์•˜๋‹ค. ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ๊ณผ ์œ ์˜ํ•œ ์ƒ๊ด€๊ด€๊ณ„๋ฅผ ๋ณด์ธ ํ”ผ์งˆ ํด๋Ÿฌ์Šคํ„ฐ ์ค‘ ์ขŒ๋ฐ˜๊ตฌ์˜ inferior temporal, entorhinal, parahippocampal, fusiform, and lingual์„ ํฌํ•จํ•˜๋Š” ํด๋Ÿฌ์Šคํ„ฐ์˜ ํ”ผ์งˆ ๋‘๊ป˜๋Š” ์ „๋ฐ˜์  ์ธ์ง€๊ธฐ๋Šฅ ๋ฐ ์–ธ์–ด๊ธฐ์–ต๊ธฐ๋Šฅ๊ณผ ์—ฐ๊ด€์ด ์žˆ์—ˆ๋‹ค. ๊ฒฐ๋ก  ๋ฐ ํ•ด์„: ๊ฒฐ๋ก ์ ์œผ๋กœ ๋ณธ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๋Š”, ์‹ ์ฒด๋ถ€์ฐฉ ๋‹จ์ผ ์‚ผ์ถ•๊ฐ€์†๊ณ„๋กœ ์ธก์ •ํ•œ ๋ณดํ–‰๋ณ€์ด์„ฑ์˜ ์ธ์ง€์ €ํ•˜ ์œ„ํ—˜ ์˜ˆ์ธก ๋””์ง€ํ„ธ ๋ฐ”์ด์˜ค๋งˆ์ปค๋กœ์„œ์˜ ๊ฐ€๋Šฅ์„ฑ์— ๊ทผ๊ฑฐ๋ฅผ ์ œ์‹œํ•˜๊ณ  ์žˆ๋‹ค.Background and Objectives: Large public health burden of dementia and the absence of a cure highlight the need for early identification of those at risk for cognitive decline or dementia to prevent and/or delay the onset of dementia. Emerging evidence indicates gait variability, the fluctuation of a gait measure from one step to the next, strongly relate to the risk of cognitive decline, MCI and dementia. Gait variability obtain via wearable sensor is a promising digital biomarker for predicting risk of cognitive impairment due to its favorable practical advantages of being able to obtain measurements over a longer period of time under unsupervised real-world conditions at lower cost. In my thesis, I examine the possibility that gait variability measured by a single body-worn tri-axial accelerometer (TAA) can be used as a digital biomarker to predict future risk of cognitive decline. In the first study, I examined whether gait variability obtained by the body-worn TAA could predict future risk of cognitive decline in older people with normal cognition (NC). In the second study, I then identify neural substrates that theoretically support the potential of gait variability as a digital biomarker in older adults with larger sample size and broader range of cognitive function. Additionally, I hypothesized higher gait variability would be related to lower cortical thickness, especially in regions important for cognitive function and memory, and that these regions would represent a shared neural substrate for gait control and cognitive impairment. Methods: In the study I, we conducted 4-year prospective cohort study on 91 community-dwelling cognitively normal elderly individuals without cerebral ischemic burden or Parkinsonism. We evaluated gait speed and step time variability using a TAA placed on the center of body mass, and diagnosed mild cognitive impairment (MCI) according to the International Working Group on MCI. We performed Kaplan-Meier analysis with consecutive log-rank testing for MCI-free survival by cohort-specific tertiles of gait variability; hazard ratios (HR) of incident MCI were estimated using Cox proportional hazards regression analysis adjusted for age, sex, education level, Cumulative Illness Rating Scale score, GDS score, and presence of the apolipoprotein E ฮต4 allele. In the study II, we cross-sectionally investigated the cortical and subcortical neural structures associated with gait variability, and the shared neural substrates of gait variability and cognitive function in 207 non-demented older adults. We obtained the cortical thickness and subcortical volumes from the magnetic resonance images, and examined associations between gait variability, cognitive function, and cortical thickness and subcortical volumes. Finally, we analyzed the mediation effect of the cluster cortical thickness and subcortical volume which had a significant association with both gait variability and cognitive function on the association between gait variability and cognition. Results: In the study I, subjects with high gait variability showed about 12-fold higher risk of MCI (HR = 11.97, 95% CI = 1.29โ€“111.37) than those with mid-to-low variability. However, those with slow gait speed showed comparable MCI risk to those with mid-to-high speed (HR = 5.04, 95% CI = 0.53โ€“ 48.18). We additionally found that no sex differences were found when assessing the ability of high gait variability to predict future cognitive decline. When we computed gait variability and gait speed as continuous variables to explore whether there are any threshold effects, the risk of incident cognitive decline increased 1.5 times per 10% increment of gait variability, whereas it did not change significantly with changes of gait speed. In the study II, higher gait variability was associated with lower cognitive functions. We found the widespread decrease in cortical thickness with increasing gait variability while there was no significant association with the volume of subcortical structures. Among the clusters that showed significant correlation with the gait variability, a cluster that included the inferior temporal, entorhinal, parahippocampal, fusiform, and lingual in left hemisphere was also associated with global cognitive function, and verbal memory function. Cortical thickness of the cluster explained 17% of the total effect of gait variability on global cognitive function measured by CERAD-TS. Interpretation: Gait variability measured by a single body-worn TAA could be a novel digital biomarker of risk of cognitive decline that could be used repeatedly and frequently and at low cost to test risk of individuals without clinical evidence of cognitive impairments.I Introduction 10 1. Study background 11 2. Purpose of research 16 II Methods 18 1. Study 1: Can gait variability predict the risk of cognitive decline in cognitively normal elderly? 19 1.1. Study population 19 1.2. Clinical assessments 20 1.3. Gait Assessments 22 1.4. Statistical analysis 23 2. Study 2: Shared Neural Substrates between Gait Variability-Cognitive Function 24 2.1. Study population 24 2.2. Assessments of cognition and medical conditions 25 2.3. Gait assessments 26 2.4. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) acquisition and preprocessing 27 2.5. Statistical analyses 28 III Results 32 1. Study 1: Can gait variability predict the risk of cognitive decline in cognitively normal elderly? 33 1.1. Association of gait variability and gait speed status with the risk of MCI 34 2. Study 2: Shared Neural Substrates between Gait Variability-Cognitive Function 35 IV Discussion 38 [Figure 1] Risk of incident mild cognitive impairment over 4 years stratified by gait speed (a) and variability (b) by log-rank test 49 [Figure 2] Cortical thickness and gait variability in non-demented older adults 50 [Figure 3] Cortical thickness of LH1 cluster mediates effect of gait variability on CERAD-TS (a) and VMS (b) 51 [Table 1] Demographic, clinical, cognitive function, and gait characteristics of the subjects 52 [Table 2] Prediction of mild cognitive impairment in cognitively normal elderly individuals 54 [Table 3] Characteristics of participants 55 [Table 4] Vertex-Wise Analyses of Gait Variability and Cortical Thickness 56 [Table 5] Regression Analyses of Gait Variability and Cortical Thickness 57 [Table 6] Associations between Cortical Regions related with Gait Variability and Cognitive Function 58 Bibliography 59 ๊ฐ์‚ฌ์˜ ๊ธ€ 66 ์ดˆ ๋ก 67๋ฐ•

    Rumination and executive dysfunction: Risk factors for vascular depression

    Get PDF
    Introduction: The widely-supported vascular depression hypothesis is underspecified with respect to cognitive mechanisms by which high cerebrovascular burden (CVB) and neuropathology relate to depressive symptoms. Integration of the vascular depression hypothesis with the CaR-FA-X model, a framework of affect regulation mechanisms, suggest that Rumination (R) and executive dysfunction (X) may increase due to altered recruitment of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex resulting from high CVB and underlying neuropathology. This process would contribute to depressive symptomatology among older adults with high CVB. The progression of examined hypotheses included mediation models examining mechanistic relationships between predictors (CVB, DLPFC activation), cognitive correlates (rumination, executive functioning), and affective outcomes (depressive symptoms). Method: A sample of 52 community-dwelling, stroke-free, individuals over the age of 70, without history of severe mental illness, dementia, or severe cognitive impairment, completed the Ruminative Responses Scale, provided self-reported cerebrovascular burden data (cardiac disease, hypertension, diabetes, high cholesterol), and completed executive function tasks (Stroop, Flanker) while their hemodynamic response was measured using fNIRS. The Geriatric Depression Scale was used to assess depressive symptomatology. Prefrontal cortical recruitment was assessed using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Results: A progression of conventional and bootstrapped regression-based models broadly supported relationships between CVB and depressive symptoms, but not between DLPFC activation and depressive symptoms. No mechanistic relationships were found, with respect to analyses testing prospective cognitive mediators. Conclusions: Primary findings from this study indicate that cerebrovascular burden predicts depressive symptomatology among older adults and is related to a reduction in inhibitory control ability. Further, these findings inform CVB measurement and mental health implications of contrasting approaches to CVB measurement. A primary contribution of this thesis is that results appear to support utilization of fNIRS, a low-cost and accessible neuroimaging paradigm, for the study of lateralized cognition among older adults

    Moderating Effect of Cortical Thickness on BOLD Signal Variability Age-Related Changes

    Get PDF
    The time course of neuroanatomical structural and functional measures across the lifespan is commonly reported in association with aging. Blood oxygen-level dependent signal variability, estimated using the standard deviation of the signal, or โ€œBOLDSD,โ€ is an emerging metric of variability in neural processing, and has been shown to be positively correlated with cognitive flexibility. Generally, BOLDSD is reported to decrease with aging, and is thought to reflect age-related cognitive decline. Additionally, it is well established that normative aging is associated with structural changes in brain regions, and that these predict functional decline in various cognitive domains. Nevertheless, the interaction between alterations in cortical morphology and BOLDSD changes has not been modeled quantitatively. The objective of the current study was to investigate the influence of cortical morphology metrics [i.e., cortical thickness (CT), gray matter (GM) volume, and cortical area (CA)] on age-related BOLDSD changes by treating these cortical morphology metrics as possible physiological confounds using linear mixed models. We studied these metrics in 28 healthy older subjects scanned twice at approximately 2.5 years interval. Results show that BOLDSD is confounded by cortical morphology metrics. Respectively, changes in CT but not GM volume nor CA, show a significant interaction with BOLDSD alterations. Our study highlights that CT changes should be considered when evaluating BOLDSD alternations in the lifespan

    Neuroprotective Effects of Cardiorespiratory Fitness on White Matter Integrity and Cognition Across the Adult Lifespan

    Get PDF
    Objective: Cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) is associated with decreased risk for cognitive decline. Accumulating evidence has linked CRF to more conserved white matter (WM) integrity and better cognitive performance in older adults. Additional research is needed to determine: (1) which WM tracts are most strongly related to CRF, (2) whether CRF-related benefits on WM translate to enhanced executive functioning (EF), and (3) if the neuroprotective effects of CRF are age-dependent. This study aimed to evaluate CRF as an intervention for modulating decreased WM integrity and EF in aging. Method: Participants were community-dwelling adults (N = 499; ages 20-85) from the open-access Nathan Kline Institute โ€“ Rockland Sample (NKIRS) with CRF (bike test), self-report of physical activity, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), and EF data. Mixed-effect modeling tested the interaction between CRF and age on WM integrity (global and local microstructure). Significant WM tracts were retained for structural equation modeling to determine whether enhanced microstructure mediated a positive relationship between CRF and EF. Results: Among older participants (age 60), CRF was significantly related to stronger whole-brain (z-score slope = 0.11) and local WM integrity within five tracts (z-score slope range = 0.14 โ€“ 0.20). In support of the age-dependent hypothesis, the CRFโ€“WM relationship was comparably weaker (z-score slopes 0.11) and more limited (one WM tract) in younger adults. CRF was more consistently related to WM than self-report of physical activity. Although CRF was linked to enhanced WM integrity, its potential benefits on EF were not directly observed. Conclusion: The findings highlight the importance of positive lifestyle factors, such as physical activity, in maintaining brain health in senescence. CRF may selectively preserve a collection of anterior and posterior WM connections related to visuomotor function

    Autonomic function in amnestic and non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment : spectral heart rate variability analysis provides evidence for a brain–heart axis

    Get PDF
    Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a heterogeneous syndrome with two main clinical subtypes, amnestic (aMCI) and non-amnestic (naMCI). The analysis of heart rate variability (HRV) is a tool to assess autonomic function. Cognitive and autonomic processes are linked via the central autonomic network. Autonomic dysfunction entails several adverse outcomes. However, very few studies have investigated autonomic function in MCI and none have considered MCI subtypes or the relationship of HRV indices with different cognitive domains and structural brain damage. We assessed autonomic function during an active orthostatic challenge in 253 oupatients aged\u2009 65\u200965, [n\u2009=\u200982 aMCI, n\u2009=\u200993 naMCI, n\u2009=\u200978 cognitively normal (CN), neuropsychologically tested] with power spectral analysis of HRV. We used visual rating scales to grade cerebrovascular burden and hippocampal/insular atrophy (HA/IA) on neuroimaging. Only aMCI showed a blunted response to orthostasis. Postural changes in normalised low frequency (LF) power and in the LF to high frequency ratio correlated with a memory test (positively) and HA/IA (negatively) in aMCI, and with attention/executive function tests (negatively) and cerebrovascular burden (positively) in naMCI. These results substantiate the view that the ANS is differentially impaired in aMCI and naMCI, consistently with the neuroanatomic substrate of Alzheimer's and small-vessel subcortical ischaemic disease

    Endothelial Function Is Associated with White Matter Microstructure and Executive Function in Older Adults

    Get PDF
    Age-related declines in endothelial function can lead to cognitive decline. However, little is known about the relationships between endothelial function and specific neurocognitive functions. This study explored the relationship between measures of endothelial function (reactive hyperemia index; RHI), white matter (WM) health (fractional anisotropy, FA, and WM hyperintensity volume, WMH), and executive function (Trail Making Test (TMT); Trail B - Trail A). Participants were 36 older adults between the ages of 59 and 69 (mean age = 63.89 years, SD = 2.94). WMH volume showed no relationship with RHI or executive function. However, there was a positive relationship between RHI and FA in the genu and body of the corpus callosum. In addition, higher RHI and FA were each associated with better executive task performance. Tractography was used to localize the WM tracts associated with RHI to specific portions of cortex. Results indicated that the RHI-FA relationship observed in the corpus callosum primarily involved tracts interconnecting frontal regions, including the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and frontopolar cortex, linked with executive function. These findings suggest that superior endothelial function may help to attenuate age-related declines in WM microstructure in portions of the corpus callosum that interconnect prefrontal brain regions involved in executive function
    • โ€ฆ
    corecore