341 research outputs found
Zien en bewegen
REDE
UITGESPROKEN BIJ DE AANVAARDING VAN
HET AMBT VAN GEWOON LEKTOR IN DE
FYSIOLOGIE AAN DE MEDISCHE FACULTEIT TE
ROTTERDAM OP DONDERDAG 16 APRIL 197
Early components of the human vestibulo-ocular response to head rotation: latency and gain
To characterize vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR) properties in the time
window in which contributions by other systems are minimal, eye movements
during the first 50-100 ms after the start of transient angular head
accelerations ( approximately 1000 degrees /s(2)) imposed by a torque
helmet were analyzed in normal human subjects. Orientations of the head
and both eyes were recorded with magnetic search coils (resolution,
approximately 1 min arc; 1000 samples/s). Typically, the first response to
a head perturbation was an anti-compensatory eye movement with zero
latency, peak-velocity of several degrees per second, and peak excursion
of several tenths of a degree. This was interpreted as a passive
mechanical response to linear acceleration of the orbital tissues caused
by eccentric rotation of the eye. The response was modeled as a damped
oscillation (approximately 13 Hz) of the orbital contents, approaching a
constant eye deviation for a sustained linear acceleration. The subsequent
compensatory eye movements showed (like the head movements) a linear
increase in velocity, which allowed estimates of latency and gain with
linear regressions. After appropriate accounting for the preceding passive
eye movements, average VOR latency (for pooled eyes, directions, and
subjects) was calculated as 8.6 ms. Paired comparisons between the two
eyes revealed that the latency for the eye contralateral to the direction
of head rotation was, on average, 1.3 ms shorter than for the ipsilateral
eye. This highly significant average inter-ocular difference was
attributed to the additional internuclear abducens neuron in the pathway
to the ipsilateral eye. Average acceleration gain (ratio between slopes of
eye and head velocities) over the first 40-50 ms was approximately 1.1.
Instantaneous velocity gain, calculated as Veye(t)/Vhead(t-latency),
showed a gradual build-up converging toward unity (often after a slight
overshoot). Instantaneous acceleration gain also converged toward unity
but showed a much steeper build-up and larger oscillations. This behavior
of acceleration and velocity gain could be accounted for by modeling the
eye movements as the sum of the passive response to the linear
acceleration and the active rotational VOR. Due to the latency and the
anticompensatory component, gaze stabilization was never complete. The
influence of visual targets was limited. The initial VOR was identical
with a distant target (continuously visible or interrupted) and in
complete darkness. A near visual target caused VOR gain to rise to a
higher level, but the time after which the difference between far and near
targets emerged varied between individuals
Nonconjugate adaptation of human saccades to anisometropic spectacles: Meridian-specificity
Abstract
Recently it has been demonstrated that saccades become different in size in the two eyes if a subject is adapted to anisometropic spectacles, which provide visual images of different magnitude to the two eyes. These nonconjugate adaptations adequately meet the requirements of those spectacles and, once acquired, they persist (with some reduction) even during monocular viewing. We now demonstrate that such nonconjugate adaptations of saccades can be meridian-specific, if there is a pressure for such meridian-specificity. This pressure was provided by means of a cylindrical spectacle-lens. Adaptations along a vertical, horizontal or oblique meridian did not transfer to the orthogonal meridian. These results demonstrate a capability of saccadic adaptation to deal with calibration problems restricted not only to one eye, but even to one specific plane of muscular action. Our results also suggest that the meridian-specific adaptations of oblique saccades take place at a stage before the decomposition of motor commands into separate horizontal and vertical components. The meridian-specific nonconjugacies were also expressed in smooth-pursuit eye movements. Post-saccadic drift adapted only along the horizontal meridian
Eye torsion associated with disparity-induced vertical vergence in humans
Recently, Enright described an unexpected association between disparity-induced vertical vergence and cycloversion (conjugate eye torsion) [Enright (1992)Vision Research, 415, 279]. The present experiments were performed to verify these findings and investigate the nature of this association. We presented subjects with a dichoptic image of concentric circles in which a step in vertical disparity of 1 deg was introduced. After 4 sec the disparity was eliminated. Eye movements were measured with scierai coils. We confirmed Enright's findings in that a left-over right vertical vergence was associated with levo-cycloversion (upper poles towards left shoulder) and vice versa. The size of the cycloversion and the vertical vergence were in the same range. In addition we found that part of the cycloversion response was in the form of a torsional nystagmus and that the relative contribution of the left and right eyes was independent of the horizontal gaze angle. These additional findings are in conflict with the hypothesis, offered by Enright, that the association is caused by a bilateral activity of the superior oblique muscles
Voluntary binocular gaze-shifts in the plane of regard: Dynamics of version and vergence
We studied the dynamics of voluntary, horizontal, binocular gaze-shifts between pairs of continuously visible, real three-dimensional targets. Subjects were stabilized on a biteboard to allow full control of target angles, which were made to differ only in distance (pure vergence), only in direction (pure version; conjugate saccades) or in both distance and direction (disjunctive saccades). A wide range of changes in vergence (0-25 deg) and version (0-65 deg) was recorded to study the dynamics of disjunctive saccades, described until now for limited ranges, throughout the horizontal oculomotor range within manual working space, and to study the velocity-duration-amplitude relations ("main sequence") of disjunctive vs conjugate saccades. Pure vergence was almost never observed; divergence, especially, was always associated with saccades. Likewise, horizontal saccades were never strictly conjugate, they always contained a transient divergence-convergence sequence. The amplitude and velocity of these transient components varied systematically with saccadic size. In combined version-vergence movements, vergence was, in general, accelerated and shortened as a function of increasing version. This effect was fairly uniform for divergence, which appeared to increase in velocity by about as much as the transient peak divergent velocity of the version saccade. The intrasaccadic fraction of divergence increased from about 50% to close to 100% as a function of increasing version. For convergence, saccades up to about 20 deg were also accelerating; in this case it appeared as if the transient peak convergent velocity of the version saccade was added to the basic convergence velocity. For larger saccades this effect was partly counteracted by the penetration of an initial divergence associated with the saccade. This initial divergence delayed and slowed down convergence. The intrasaccadic fraction of convergence varied between about 40% and 70%. In disjunctive saccades the individual eyes did not follow the main-sequence parameters of conjugate saccades of comparable sizes, except for the eye that moved with the combination "abduction and divergence". For all other combinations of vergence and version, disjunctive saccades had lower peak velocities and longer durations than conjugate saccades. As a consequence, disjunctive version was also slower than conjugate version. Thus, while version accelerates vergence, vergence slows down version: in the generalized case of three-dimensional gaze-shifts, peak velocities and durations are in between those of the limiting cases of pure version and pure vergence. We conclude that, within manual working space, binocular gaze-shifts are effected by the highly integrated action of conjugate and disjunctive mechanisms, both of which are expressed preferentially in fast, saccadic movements
Instability of ocular torsion during fixation: Cyclovergence is more stable than cycloversion
We investigated spontaneous variation of binocular torsion. Variation was expressed as SD of torsional eye positions measured over periods up to 32 sec. Subjects viewed a single dot target for periods of 32 sec. In half of the trials a large random-dot background pattern was superimposed on the dot. The movements of both eyes were measured with scleral induction coils. Spontaneous torsional movements were largely conjugate: cyclovergence was much more stable than cycloversion. This difference was not due to roll head movements. Stability of cyclovergence was improved by the background pattern. Although overall stability (SD of position) of cycloversion was unaffected by a background, the background induced or enhanced a small-amplitude torsional nystagmus in 3 out of 4 subjects. We hypothesize that the difference in stability of cycloversion vs cyclovergence reflects the greater importance of torsional retinal correspondence, compared to absolute torsional position. In two subjects we found evidence for the existence of cyclophoria, manifested by systematic shifts in cyclovergence caused by the appearance and disappearance of the background
Eye torsion elicited by oscillating gratings: Effects of orientation, wavelength and stationary contours
We studied binocular cyclorotatory (torsional) eye movements in response to gratings that oscillated sinusoidally in a frontal plane. The square-wave gratings viewed by the right and left eye were presented and controlled separately to induce cycloversion and cyclovergence by oscillation in phase and out of phase. Eye movements were recorded with scierai induction coils. Stimulus oscillation frequency ranged from 0.125 to 1 Hz and the wavelength of die gratings ranged from 0.92 to 25.75 deg of visual angle. Cycloversion and cyclovergence gain were, on average, comparable in magnitude and decreased with increasing oscillation frequency. There was no consistent effect of the wavelength on the magnitude of the responses. In general, responses were considerably higher to gratings that were oriented horizontally than to those oriented vertically. This anisotropy was present both in cycloversion and cyclovergence. It was enhanced in a larger sized stimulus and by presenting stationary, orthogonal contours (mimicking a “shear” movement), but it was not consistently influenced by wavelength. Cyclovergence showed a phase lag, which increased with oscillation frequency but which was independent of wavelength. In contrast, cycloversion showed a slight phase lead which was independent of bot
Interocular yoking in human saccades examined by mutual information analysis
International audienceABSTRACT : BACKGROUND : Saccadic eye movements align the two eyes precisely to foveate a target. Trial-by-trial variance of eye movement is always observed within an identical experimental condition. This has often been treated as experimental error without addressing its significance. The present study examined statistical linkages between the two eyes' movements, namely interocular yoking, for the variance of eye position and velocity. METHODS : Horizontal saccadic movements were recorded from twelve right-eye-dominant subjects while they decided on saccade direction in Go-Only sessions and on both saccade execution and direction in Go/NoGo sessions. We used infrared corneal reflection to record simultaneously and independently the movement of each eye. Quantitative measures of yoking were provided by mutual information analysis of eye position or velocity, which is sensitive to both linear and non-linear relationships between the eyes' movements. Our mutual information analysis relied on the variance of the eyes movements in each experimental condition. The range of movements for each eye varies for different conditions so yoking was further studied by comparing GO-Only vs. Go/NoGo sessions, leftward vs. rightward saccades. RESULTS : Mutual information analysis showed that velocity yoking preceded positional yoking. Cognitive load increased trial variances of velocity with no increase in velocity yoking, suggesting that cognitive load may alter neural processes in areas to which oculomotor control is not tightly linked. The comparison between experimental conditions showed that interocular linkage in velocity variance of the right eye lagged that of the left eye during saccades. CONCLUSIONS : We conclude quantitative measure of interocular yoking based on trial-to-trial variance within a condition, as well as variance between conditions, provides a powerful tool for studying the binocular movement mechanism
The function of visual search and memory in sequential looking tasks
Eye and head movements were recorded as unrestrained subjects tapped or only looked at nearby targets. Scanning patterns were the same in both tasks: subjects looked at each target before tapping it; visual search had similar speeds and gaze-shift accuracies. Looking however, took longer and, unlike tapping, benefitted little from practice. Looking speeded up more than tapping when memory load was reduced: memory was more efficient during tapping. Conclusion: eye movements made when only looking are different from those made when tapping. Visual search functions as a separate process, incorporated into both tasks: it can be used to improve performance when memory load is heavy
- …