33 research outputs found
Oblivious âSex Traffickersâ: Challenging stereotypes and the fairness of US trafficking laws
In this paper, we explore third parties who unexpectedly fell within the legal definition of a sex trafficker. The anti-trafficking lobby and media stories frequently portray traffickers as organised, psychopathic, violent, and child kidnappers. We dismantle these depictions by showing the unexpected people who qualify as traffickers. This paper incorporates findings from two studies involving eighty-five third parties in New York City and forty-nine in Chicago. We analyse how teenagers, drivers, and boyfriends qualify as traffickers under US law. We find that two-thirds of them hold inaccurate views about the difference between sex trafficking and facilitating prostitution. Trafficking can be incidental or temporary, and traffickers in these samples were often oblivious to their legal status, potentially resulting in lengthy prison sentences. We conclude by calling for differential sentencing based on traffickersâ age, and awareness campaigns designed to alert third parties of the legal distinctions between pandering and sex trafficking
LAWYERSâ PERCEPTIONS OF THE FAIRNESS OF JUDICIAL ASSISTANCE TO SELF-REPRESENTED LITIGANTS
How much assistance should a trial judge provide a self-represented litigant [SRL] before the judgeâs impartiality will be reasonably questioned? This question has been of continuing concern to both the bench and bar ever since the rise of the pro se litigation movement in the late 1990s, particularly in the context of âmixedâ cases involving an SRL and a represented party. Case law and ethics codes provide inconsistent decisions and vague guidelines for judges, who must balance their duty to provide reasonable assistance with their duty to ensure a fair trial for all parties. This paper reports the results of a survey administered to 210 Canadian family law practitioners who were presented with 16 hypothetical scenarios involving an SRL and a represented party. Respondents indicated their views regarding the impartiality and helpfulness of the trial judge in each scenario, involving various procedural defaults by the SRL and different forms of judicial assistance or lack thereof. The results indicate that lawyers' perceptions of a judge's impartiality are affected, inter alia, by the favourability of the outcome for the SRL, and whether the assistance provided dealt with procedural or substantive matters. Future research is needed to determine whether a consensus can be established regarding perceptions of lawyers, lay persons, and judges regarding which forms of assistance are reasonable and required, permissible, or impermissible.
JusquâĂ quel point un juge de premiĂšre instance peut-il venir en aide Ă une partie qui se reprĂ©sente elle-mĂȘme sans que son impartialitĂ© puisse raisonnablement ĂȘtre mise en doute? Cette question ne cesse de prĂ©occuper les juges et les avocats depuis lâessor quâa pris le phĂ©nomĂšne de lâautoreprĂ©sentation Ă la fin des annĂ©es 1990, en particulier dans le contexte des cas « mixtes », impliquant une partie qui se reprĂ©sente elle-mĂȘme et une partie reprĂ©sentĂ©e par un avocat. La jurisprudence et les codes de dĂ©ontologie fournissent des dĂ©cisions contradictoires et des lignes directrices vagues aux juges, qui doivent trouver un Ă©quilibre entre leur devoir de fournir une aide raisonnable et leur obligation dâassurer un procĂšs Ă©quitable Ă toutes les parties. Le prĂ©sent article expose les rĂ©sultats dâune enquĂȘte rĂ©alisĂ©e auprĂšs de 210 spĂ©cialistes du droit de la famille du Canada, auxquels on a soumis 16 scĂ©narios hypothĂ©tiques impliquant une partie se reprĂ©sentant elle-mĂȘme et une partie reprĂ©sentĂ©e par un avocat. Les rĂ©pondants ont indiquĂ© leur point de vue quant Ă lâimpartialitĂ© et Ă lâaide accordĂ©e par le juge de premiĂšre instance dans chacun des scĂ©narios. Les scĂ©narios comportaient diverses erreurs de procĂ©dure commises par la partie se reprĂ©sentant elle-mĂȘme et diffĂ©rentes formes dâaide judiciaire ou lâabsence dâaide de cette nature. Les rĂ©sultats indiquent que la façon dont les avocats perçoivent lâimpartialitĂ© dâun juge est affectĂ©e, entre autres, par la mesure dans laquelle lâissue est favorable pour la partie se reprĂ©sentant elle-mĂȘme et par le fait que lâaide a portĂ© sur des procĂ©dures ou sur des questions de fond. Il faudra dâautres recherches pour dĂ©terminer si un consensus peut ĂȘtre atteint relativement Ă la façon dont les avocats, les non-initiĂ©s et les juges perçoivent les formes dâaide qui sont raisonnables et requises, autorisĂ©es ou interdites
LAWYERSâ PERCEPTIONS OF THE FAIRNESS OF JUDICIAL ASSISTANCE TO SELF-REPRESENTED LITIGANTS
How much assistance should a trial judge provide a self-represented litigant [SRL] before the judgeâs impartiality will be reasonably questioned? This question has been of continuing concern to both the bench and bar ever since the rise of the pro se litigation movement in the late 1990s, particularly in the context of âmixedâ cases involving an SRL and a represented party. Case law and ethics codes provide inconsistent decisions and vague guidelines for judges, who must balance their duty to provide reasonable assistance with their duty to ensure a fair trial for all parties. This paper reports the results of a survey administered to 210 Canadian family law practitioners who were presented with 16 hypothetical scenarios involving an SRL and a represented party. Respondents indicated their views regarding the impartiality and helpfulness of the trial judge in each scenario, involving various procedural defaults by the SRL and different forms of judicial assistance or lack thereof. The results indicate that lawyers' perceptions of a judge's impartiality are affected, inter alia, by the favourability of the outcome for the SRL, and whether the assistance provided dealt with procedural or substantive matters. Future research is needed to determine whether a consensus can be established regarding perceptions of lawyers, lay persons, and judges regarding which forms of assistance are reasonable and required, permissible, or impermissible.
JusquâĂ quel point un juge de premiĂšre instance peut-il venir en aide Ă une partie qui se reprĂ©sente elle-mĂȘme sans que son impartialitĂ© puisse raisonnablement ĂȘtre mise en doute? Cette question ne cesse de prĂ©occuper les juges et les avocats depuis lâessor quâa pris le phĂ©nomĂšne de lâautoreprĂ©sentation Ă la fin des annĂ©es 1990, en particulier dans le contexte des cas « mixtes », impliquant une partie qui se reprĂ©sente elle-mĂȘme et une partie reprĂ©sentĂ©e par un avocat. La jurisprudence et les codes de dĂ©ontologie fournissent des dĂ©cisions contradictoires et des lignes directrices vagues aux juges, qui doivent trouver un Ă©quilibre entre leur devoir de fournir une aide raisonnable et leur obligation dâassurer un procĂšs Ă©quitable Ă toutes les parties. Le prĂ©sent article expose les rĂ©sultats dâune enquĂȘte rĂ©alisĂ©e auprĂšs de 210 spĂ©cialistes du droit de la famille du Canada, auxquels on a soumis 16 scĂ©narios hypothĂ©tiques impliquant une partie se reprĂ©sentant elle-mĂȘme et une partie reprĂ©sentĂ©e par un avocat. Les rĂ©pondants ont indiquĂ© leur point de vue quant Ă lâimpartialitĂ© et Ă lâaide accordĂ©e par le juge de premiĂšre instance dans chacun des scĂ©narios. Les scĂ©narios comportaient diverses erreurs de procĂ©dure commises par la partie se reprĂ©sentant elle-mĂȘme et diffĂ©rentes formes dâaide judiciaire ou lâabsence dâaide de cette nature. Les rĂ©sultats indiquent que la façon dont les avocats perçoivent lâimpartialitĂ© dâun juge est affectĂ©e, entre autres, par la mesure dans laquelle lâissue est favorable pour la partie se reprĂ©sentant elle-mĂȘme et par le fait que lâaide a portĂ© sur des procĂ©dures ou sur des questions de fond. Il faudra dâautres recherches pour dĂ©terminer si un consensus peut ĂȘtre atteint relativement Ă la façon dont les avocats, les non-initiĂ©s et les juges perçoivent les formes dâaide qui sont raisonnables et requises, autorisĂ©es ou interdites
Superiority of the Left Hemisphere in the Recognition of Emotional Faces
Thirty-two right-handed university students (16 males and 16 females) participated in a choice reaction time study assessing the perception of hemispheric differences in positive (happiness, surprise) and negative (anger, disgust) facial affect. Subjects were exposed to slides of facial expressions in either the left or right visual field, and latency of affect identification was measured by having each subject move a lever forward or backward in a specified direction to indicate the emotion perceived. An analysis of variance with repeated measures over hand used, visual field, and affect across sex of subject revealed significant main effects for visual field, affect and sex of subject. In contrast to prior studies, the emotional faces were perceived faster in the right visual field than in the left visual field. This finding is explained in terms of the more demanding analytical task chosen in contrast to previous research which typically employed a holistic matching or recognition task. The faster perception of positive affect may be attributed to the increased salience of the facial cues present in those faces displaying happiness or surprise. Finally, the faster response of males supports existing data suggesting greater cerebral lateralization in the male brain
Victim-Offender Overlap: Risky Spaces, Deviant Subculture, and Revenge Motives in Committing Fraud in Cyberspace
This study contributes to understanding victim-offender overlap (Berg & Mulford, 2020) in cyber fraud scams. Adults (N = 476) completed a Qualtrics online survey. Online peer encouragement and perceiving themselves as a target were consistent predictors across all three measures of victim-offender overlap and predicted perpetrating cyber fraud scams after controlling for low self-control and prior victimization. The offender first group were more likely to see themselves as a target and feel vengeful and were less likely to experience emotional distress or feel defeated than the victim first group. Social learning and emotions contribute to the victim-offender overlap
Formation of beliefs about criminal events and punishment: Contextual factors and biased recall.
Formation of beliefs about criminal events and punishment: Contextual factors and biased recall
Hemispheric Differences in the Perception of Positive and Negative Faces
Thirty-two right-handed subjects (15 males and 17 females) participated in a study investigating the effects of the perception of positive and negative emotional stimuli on choice reaction time. Slides of faces showing positive (happy, surprise) or negative (anger, disgust, sadness) affect were presented via a tachistoscope to either the right or left visual field. A 2 Ă 2 Ă 2 Ă 2 mixed analysis of variance with repeated measures over the factors of hand used by visual field by affect across sex of subject revealed a main effect for visual field (with slightly faster responses to stimuli presented in the right visual field) and a strong affect by visual field interaction with positive facial slides identified more quickly than negative facial slides when presented to the right visual field
Reward Allocation by Impartial Allocators to Friend or Stranger Co-Workers Under Equal and Unequal Ability and Performance
The effects of a relationship variable (friends, strangers), an ability variable (equal, unequal), and a performance variable (equal, unequal) were studied in 40 male and 40 female university students. Eight versions of a hypothetical vignette were prepared to manipulate the eight treatment combinations. After reading an assigned vignette, subjects were asked to allocate $90 between two stranger or friend co-workers. Among other effects, the Relationship by Ability by Performance interaction was significant. The stranger, unequal ability, unequal performance allocations to the lower ability co-worker were significantly higher than the stranger, equal ability, unequal performance allocations, and the stranger, unequal ability, unequal performance allocations were higher than the friends, unequal ability, unequal performance allocations; this difference approached significance (p\u3c.06). The questionnaire data revealed that, despite having favored strangers, the impartial allocators expected strangers to be less satisfied with their distribution than friends with their distribution. In this study, impartial allocators tended to compensate strangers to a greater extent for low performance or ability than friends or, alternatively, were more likely to ignore performance and ability differences between strangers than between friends