83 research outputs found

    Tendências genéticas e fenotípicas para características produtivas e reprodutivas para fêmeas da raça Nelore.

    Get PDF
    Objetivou-se estimar as tendências genéticas e fenotípicas, utilizando Inferência Bayesiana, para um indicador de produtividade e para características reprodutiva e produtivas para fêmeas da raça Nelore. O arquivo de trabalho estava constituído de 3.029 registros de animais, filhas de 357 touros e 3.029 vacas. Foi adotado um modelo animal tetracaracterística, considerando como aleatórios os efeitos genéticos aditivos diretos e os residuais e como fixos o efeitos do ano de nascimento e do grupo de contemporâneos. As tendências genéticas diretas para o peso aos 365 e aos 550 dias de idade, para a idade da vaca ao parto e para o indicador de produtividade total, foram: 101,1 e 126,2 g/ano; -1,10 dias/ano e 116,2 g/bezerro desmamado/ano, respectivamente. As tendências fenotípicas foram desfavoráveis para todas as características avaliadas. As tendências genéticas obtidas para as características avaliadas indicam que a seleção realizada vem promovendo progresso genético; contudo, melhorias nas condições de ambiente devem ser implementadas, para que o genótipo dos indivíduos se expresse com maior eficiência

    Estimação de parâmetros genéticos sob enfoque bayesiano para indicadores de produtividade na raça Nelore.

    Get PDF
    Objetivou-se com este trabalho estimar os componentes de variâncias, utilizando análise bayesiana multicaracterística, de um indicador de produtividade e de características produtivas e reprodutivas para fêmeas da raça Nelore. O arquivo estava constituído de 3.029 registros de animais, filhos de 357 touros e 3.029 vacas, nascidos no período de 1976 a 2001. Estimaram-se os parâmetros genéticos, sob enfoque Bayesiano, utilizando os programas REMUNF90 e GIBBS2F90, sob modelo animal tetra-característica que apresentava como aleatórios os efeitos genéticos aditivos diretos e os residuais e como fixos o efeitos do ano de nascimento e o efeito do grupo de contemporâneos. Observou-se que os valores pontuais para os coeficientes de herdabilidades apresentaram pequena variação em relação aos valores mínimos e máximos, seguindo a tendência das variâncias aditivas, sendo também evidenciadas graficamente. As magnitudes das estimativas de herdabilidade do efeito genético direto obtidas para o peso aos 365 e aos 550 dias de idade, para a idade ao primeiro parto e para o índice de produtividade total foram respectivamente: 0,48; 0,60; 0,37 e 0,24; sugerindo que parte considerável da variação existente entre os animais, para estas características, está sob influência de componente genético aditivo; deste modo, todas as características abordadas no presente trabalho, podem responder de forma satisfatória à seleção.Zootec 2010

    Abordagem bayesiana multivariada para características de crescimento, fertilidade e escores visuais de rebanhos da raça Brangus.

    Get PDF
    O objetivo deste trabalho foi estimar parâmetros genéticos e tendências genéticas e fenotípicas de uma população da raça Brangus. As características peso, circunferência escrotal e escores visuais de conformação, precocidade, musculatura e umbigo, padronizadas para 550 dias de idade, foram avaliadas a partir de 6.789 registros de animais nascido de 288 touros e 5.949 vacas, entre 1991 e 2001, em 49 fazendas localizadas nas regiões Centro‑Oeste, Sudeste e Sul do Brasil. Para a estimação dos parâmetros, das correlações e das tendências genéticas, foi adotado o modelo animal linear‑limiar hexacaracterística. As estimativas de herdabilidade direta foram de 0,39 e 0,27, para peso e circunferência escrotal, respectivamente, e de 0,22, 0,20, 0,23 e 0,33 para conformação, precocidade, musculatura e umbigo, o que indica considerável variação genética aditiva e que é possível obter ganho genético por meio da seleção. As correlações genéticas entre peso e circunferência escrotal com os escores de conformação, precocidade e musculatura mostram a possibilidade de resposta correlacionada. As tendências genéticas estimadas indicam grande contribuição de fontes de variação não genéticas para todas as características no período estudado, e apontam a necessidade de melhoria das condições ambientais, para que os animais expressem todo seu potencial genético

    Classical nonlinear models to describe the growth curve for Murrah buffalo breed.

    Get PDF
    With the objective of to adjust nonlinear models for the growth curves for a buffaloes herd raised in floodable lands in Rio Grande do Sul state, monthly records measured from birth to two years-old of 64 males and 63 females born between 1982 and 1989 were used. The models used were: Von Bertalanffy, Brody, Gompertz and Logistic. The parameters were estimated by NLIN procedure and the criteria used to evaluate the adjustment given by the models were: asymptotic standard deviation; coefficient of determination; average absolute deviation of residues and asymptotic index. Von Bertalanffy and Brody models overestimated the male asymptotic weight (A) in 15.9 and 171.3kg, respectively, and the Gompertz and Logistic models underestimated it in 4.5 and 13.4kg, respectively. For females, the Logistic model underestimated the asymptotic weight (-2.09kg), and Gompertz, Von Bertalanffy and Brody overestimated this parameter in 8.04, 17.7, and 280.33kg, respectively. The biggest average deviation was estimated by Brody model for both sexes, characterizing the biggest index. Considering the criteria, it is recommended the Gompertz and Logistic models for adjust females and males Murrah buffaloes breed growth curves

    Multisensory information facilitates reaction speed by enlarging activity difference between superior colliculus hemispheres in rats

    Get PDF
    Animals can make faster behavioral responses to multisensory stimuli than to unisensory stimuli. The superior colliculus (SC), which receives multiple inputs from different sensory modalities, is considered to be involved in the initiation of motor responses. However, the mechanism by which multisensory information facilitates motor responses is not yet understood. Here, we demonstrate that multisensory information modulates competition among SC neurons to elicit faster responses. We conducted multiunit recordings from the SC of rats performing a two-alternative spatial discrimination task using auditory and/or visual stimuli. We found that a large population of SC neurons showed direction-selective activity before the onset of movement in response to the stimuli irrespective of stimulation modality. Trial-by-trial correlation analysis showed that the premovement activity of many SC neurons increased with faster reaction speed for the contraversive movement, whereas the premovement activity of another population of neurons decreased with faster reaction speed for the ipsiversive movement. When visual and auditory stimuli were presented simultaneously, the premovement activity of a population of neurons for the contraversive movement was enhanced, whereas the premovement activity of another population of neurons for the ipsiversive movement was depressed. Unilateral inactivation of SC using muscimol prolonged reaction times of contraversive movements, but it shortened those of ipsiversive movements. These findings suggest that the difference in activity between the SC hemispheres regulates the reaction speed of motor responses, and multisensory information enlarges the activity difference resulting in faster responses

    A competitive integration model of exogenous and endogenous eye movements

    Get PDF
    We present a model of the eye movement system in which the programming of an eye movement is the result of the competitive integration of information in the superior colliculi (SC). This brain area receives input from occipital cortex, the frontal eye fields, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, on the basis of which it computes the location of the next saccadic target. Two critical assumptions in the model are that cortical inputs are not only excitatory, but can also inhibit saccades to specific locations, and that the SC continue to influence the trajectory of a saccade while it is being executed. With these assumptions, we account for many neurophysiological and behavioral findings from eye movement research. Interactions within the saccade map are shown to account for effects of distractors on saccadic reaction time (SRT) and saccade trajectory, including the global effect and oculomotor capture. In addition, the model accounts for express saccades, the gap effect, saccadic reaction times for antisaccades, and recorded responses from neurons in the SC and frontal eye fields in these tasks. © The Author(s) 2010

    What is ‘anti’ about anti-reaches? Reference frames selectively affect reaction times and endpoint variability

    Get PDF
    Reach movement planning involves the representation of spatial target information in different reference frames. Neurons at parietal and premotor stages of the cortical sensorimotor system represent target information in eye- or hand-centered reference frames, respectively. How the different neuronal representations affect behavioral parameters of motor planning and control, i.e. which stage of neural representation is relevant for which aspect of behavior, is not obvious from the physiology. Here, we test with a behavioral experiment if different kinematic movement parameters are affected to a different degree by either an eye- or hand-reference frame. We used a generalized anti-reach task to test the influence of stimulus-response compatibility (SRC) in eye- and hand-reference frames on reach reaction times, movement times, and endpoint variability. While in a standard anti-reach task, the SRC is identical in the eye- and hand-reference frames, we could separate SRC for the two reference frames. We found that reaction times were influenced by the SRC in eye- and hand-reference frame. In contrast, movement times were only influenced by the SRC in hand-reference frame, and endpoint variability was only influenced by the SRC in eye-reference frame. Since movement time and endpoint variability are the result of planning and control processes, while reaction times are consequences of only the planning process, we suggest that SRC effects on reaction times are highly suited to investigate reference frames of movement planning, and that eye- and hand-reference frames have distinct effects on different phases of motor action and different kinematic movement parameters

    Distributed representations of the "preparatory set" in the frontal oculomotor system: a TMS study

    Get PDF
    <p>Abstract</p> <p>Background</p> <p>The generation of saccades is influenced by the level of "preparatory set activity" in cortical oculomotor areas. This preparatory activity can be examined using the gap-paradigm in which a temporal gap is introduced between the disappearance of a central fixation target and the appearance of an eccentric target.</p> <p>Methods</p> <p>Ten healthy subjects made horizontal pro- or antisaccades in response to lateralized cues after a gap period of 200 ms. Single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) was applied to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), frontal eye field (FEF), or supplementary eye field (SEF) of the right hemisphere 100 or 200 ms after the disappearance of the fixation point. Saccade latencies were measured to probe the disruptive effect of TMS on saccade preparation. In six individuals, we gave realistic sham TMS during the gap period to mimic auditory and somatosensory stimulation without stimulating the cortex.</p> <p>Results</p> <p>TMS to DLPFC, FEF, or SEF increased the latencies of contraversive pro- and antisaccades. This TMS-induced delay of saccade initiation was particularly evident in conditions with a relatively high level of preparatory set activity: The increase in saccade latency was more pronounced at the end of the gap period and when participants prepared for prosaccades rather than antisaccades. Although the "lesion effect" of TMS was stronger with prefrontal TMS, TMS to FEF or SEF also interfered with the initiation of saccades. The delay in saccade onset induced by real TMS was not caused by non-specific effects because sham stimulation shortened the latencies of contra- and ipsiversive anti-saccades, presumably due to intersensory facilitation.</p> <p>Conclusion</p> <p>Our results are compatible with the view that the "preparatory set" for contraversive saccades is represented in a distributed cortical network, including the contralateral DLPFC, FEF and SEF.</p

    A Functional and Structural Investigation of the Human Fronto-Basal Volitional Saccade Network

    Get PDF
    Almost all cortical areas are connected to the subcortical basal ganglia (BG) through parallel recurrent inhibitory and excitatory loops, exerting volitional control over automatic behavior. As this model is largely based on non-human primate research, we used high resolution functional MRI and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) to investigate the functional and structural organization of the human (pre)frontal cortico-basal network controlling eye movements. Participants performed saccades in darkness, pro- and antisaccades and observed stimuli during fixation. We observed several bilateral functional subdivisions along the precentral sulcus around the human frontal eye fields (FEF): a medial and lateral zone activating for saccades in darkness, a more fronto-medial zone preferentially active for ipsilateral antisaccades, and a large anterior strip along the precentral sulcus activating for visual stimulus presentation during fixation. The supplementary eye fields (SEF) were identified along the medial wall containing all aforementioned functions. In the striatum, the BG area receiving almost all cortical input, all saccade related activation was observed in the putamen, previously considered a skeletomotor striatal subdivision. Activation elicited by the cue instructing pro or antisaccade trials was clearest in the medial FEF and right putamen. DTI fiber tracking revealed that the subdivisions of the human FEF complex are mainly connected to the putamen, in agreement with the fMRI findings. The present findings demonstrate that the human FEF has functional subdivisions somewhat comparable to non-human primates. However, the connections to and activation in the human striatum preferentially involve the putamen, not the caudate nucleus as is reported for monkeys. This could imply that fronto-striatal projections for the oculomotor system are fundamentally different between humans and monkeys. Alternatively, there could be a bias in published reports of monkey studies favoring the caudate nucleus over the putamen in the search for oculomotor functions
    corecore