79 research outputs found
Fever as a Cause of Hypophosphatemia in Patients with Malaria
Hypophosphatemia occurs in 40 to 60% of patients with acute malaria, and in many other conditions associated with elevations of body temperature. To determine the prevalence and causes of hypophosphatemia in patients with malaria, we retrospectively studied all adults diagnosed with acute malaria during a 12-year period. To validate our findings, we analyzed a second sample of malaria patients during a subsequent 10-year period. Serum phosphorus correlated inversely with temperature (nâ=â59, râ=ââ0.62; P<0.0001), such that each 1°C increase in body temperature was associated with a reduction of 0.18 mmol/L (0.56 mg/dL) in the serum phosphorus level (95% confidence interval: â0.12 to â0.24 mmol/L [â0.37 to â0.74 mg/dL] per 1°C). A similar effect was observed among 19 patients who had repeat measurements of serum phosphorus and temperature. In a multiple linear regression analysis, the relation between temperature and serum phosphorus level was independent of blood pH, PCO2, and serum levels of potassium, bicarbonate, calcium, albumin, and glucose. Our study demonstrates a strong inverse linear relation between body temperature and serum phosphorus level that was not explained by other factors known to cause hypophosphatemia. If causal, this association can account for the high prevalence of hypophosphatemia, observed in our patients and in previous studies of patients with malaria. Because hypophosphatemia has been observed in other clinical conditions characterized by fever or hyperthermia, this relation may not be unique to malaria. Elevation of body temperature should be added to the list of causes of hypophosphatemia
Climate change, social dreaming and art: Thinking the unthinkable
Beyond the Scientific Fact
The problem of how people can accept the reality of climate change and its effects on our daily lives is central in climate psychology. Scientific facts have proved remarkably ineffective in leading to necessary changes in lifestyle required on both an individual and a social level. For many, the facts are either traumatic or unacceptable. The requirement posed by global warming to change peopleâs deeply held desires for ever-increasing economic prosperity and the assumed concomitant wellbeing leads to shared and generalised disavowal and denial. In the world of climate change deniers or disavowers the status of scientific factual reality is a significant issue: the scientific facts backed by 97% of the scientific community are not âfact-enoughâ for meaningful social change: information, debates, surveys, focus groups and suchlike fail to open the way to significant action. In the case of climate change we are in a zone of gut rejection: even if it is, it cannot be. Al Goreâs âinconvenienceâ (Gore 2006) is more than that: it is something so inconvenient that it cannot be countenanced.
Psycho-social approaches to climate change, therefore, tend to take a containing approach to peopleâs fears, traumas and deep concerns. For example, Randall and Brownâs (2015) âcarbon conversationsâ project provides practical and experiential psycho-social approaches designed to create contained spaces for reflection and transformation. Through conversation, according to Westcott (2016), there is a chance for denial and disavowal to be converted into hope and trust, without which climate anxieties are repressed and ignored rather than confronted. Such approaches have been positively evaluated by BĂŒchs, Hinton and Smith (2015) who summarise the emotions that can be discussed through conversation related to climate change as fear and anxiety, grief, guilt, helplessness and feeling threatened in oneâs identity/status (BĂŒchs et al. 2015, p. 622). It is through the careful containment of shared conversations that people are given an opportunity to be released from the isolation, loneliness, guilt and even horror that scientific facts point to. These conversations change the nature and quality of the climate fact through each personâs relation to the facts. In a sense, the reality of the fact is given a potential for being re-experienced, almost as if it were not a factual entity in and of itself. Climate facts are thus subjectivised and their reality is found in the transactions between external and internal world experiencing.
This chapter concentrates on a different way of knowing, focusing on the shared visual and affective aspects of peopleâs relationship to climate change. It uses the data from an art and social dreaming event to explore how the use of affect-laden images in a shared âunconsciousâ context, hidden or unknown, can help us to recognize the reality of climate change. Social dreaming is a method that allows new knowledge to emerge in a gathering of people who share their dreams, associations and feelings together. The method creates a non-threatening, non-judgmental space where difficult thoughts and feelings can be expressed through images (Lawrence 2005; Manley 2014, 2018). In Social dreaming and the visual arts, the realm of worded communication is subsumed into a world of image and affect. Both involve what Donald Meltzer calls the âpoetry of the dreamâ whose role in thinking is that it âcatches and gives formal representation to the passions which are the meaning of our experience so that they may be operated upon by reasonâ (Meltzer 2009 p.47)
Expression of the KIT Protein (CD117) in Primary Cutaneous Mast Cell Tumors of the Dog
Thirty-one canine cutaneous nodules, diagnosed as mast cell tumors by histopathologic analysis on hematoxylin and eosin (HE) and toluidine blue stained sections, were used in this study to evaluate the immunohistochemical pattern of expression of KIT protein (CD117), a type III tyrosine kinase protein that is the receptor for stem cell factor (SCF), a cytokine that stimulates mast cell growth and differentiation also known as mast cell growth factor. Lesions were graded as I (well differentiated) II (intermediate differentiation) or III (poorly differentiated) according to the following morphologic features: invasiveness, cellularity and cellular morphology, mitotic index and stromal reaction. Immunohistochemical KIT expression was compared with histological grade and some histomorphologic features (cell differentiation and nuclear grade) singularly evaluated. A possible predictive role of biologic behaviour in mast cell tumors for KIT expression was also investigated. Immunohistochemical analysis revealed three different patterns of KIT expression: a cytoplasmic diffuse pattern, a membranous pattern with immunopositivity located in the cell membrane and a cytoplasmic perinuclear pattern where KIT expression was detected in the cytoplasm of the neoplastic mast cells, close to the nucleus. Statistical analysis performed by Pearson method and using the  test has showed a close relationship between different KIT immunohistochemical patterns and histological grade (P=0.00000), cell differentiation (P= 0.00000) and nuclear grade (0.0024). Kaplan & Meier estimated survival curves compared by the survival analysis revealed KIT expression significantly associated to survival time (P= 0.037) but not to cancer free interval (0.50). KIT expression may be considered a useful parameter of malignancy in cutaneous mast cell tumors just like other well known histomorphological features and showed also a predictive role of biologic behaviour of these tumors
Prognostic Value of Histologic and Immunohistochemical Features in Feline Cutaneous Mast Cell Tumors
- âŠ