1,083 research outputs found

    Does mimicking others change your self-view?

    Get PDF
    Does mimicking affect the way you think about yourself in relation to other people? In the present study, we instructed participants to either mimic or not mimic the expressions of their interaction partner. After a 3-minute interaction, participants' self-view in relation to others was measured. Results revealed that mimickers defined themselves more in relation to others than nonmimickers. Thus, mimicking others, compared to not mimicking others, changes your self-view

    The more business owners the merrier? The role of tertiary education

    Get PDF

    Parental perspectives on the awareness and delivery of preconception care

    Get PDF

    Unconscious deception detection measured by finger skin temperature and indirect veracity judgments-results of a registered report

    Get PDF
    A pre-registered experiment was conducted to examine psychophysiological responses to being lied to. Bridging research on social cognition and deception detection, we hypothesized that observing a liar compared to a truth-teller would decrease finger skin temperature of observers. Participants first watched two targets while not forewarned that they would later be asked to judge (direct and indirect) veracity, and then watched another two targets while forewarned about this. During both these phases finger skin temperature was measured. Findings pertaining to temperature partly confirmed our main hypothesis. When participants were observing a liar, irrespective of being forewarned, on average finger skin temperature declined over time. In the forewarned phase, temperature trajectories of truth-tellers were higher than those of liars, however, in the not forewarned phase, this pattern was reversed. Results confirmed our further hypotheses that participants judge liars as less likeable and less trustworthy than truth-tellers—an indication of indirect deception detection. Our hypothesis that the effect size for trustworthiness would be bigger than that of liking was not supported by the data. Additionally, and also confirming our hypothesis, participants performed around chance level when directly judging whether the target person was lying. Exploratory analyses are reported with regard to truth bias and dependency between direct and indirect veracity judgments. Limitations and directions for future work related to the existence of psychophysiological indicators of deception detection are discussed

    Development of metacognitive skills in young adolescents : a bumpy ride to the high road

    Get PDF
    This thesis shows the final results of a longitudinal project where the same participants (12 to 15 yrs.) were followed for three consecutive years. The first objective of this study was to investigate the development of both quantity and quality of metacognitive skills. The second objective was to establish whether the development of metacognitive skills is intelligence-related or relatively intelligence-independent. Finally, the generality vs. domain-specificity of developing metacognitive skills was investigated. While thinking aloud, participants performed two different tasks representing two different domains: A text-studying task and a problem-solving task. Participants__ intellectual ability was assessed, as well as their metacognitive skills and learning performance for both domains separately. In summary, this thesis has shown that (1) Metacognitive skills do increase spontaneously in frequency and quality during young adolescence, albeit not continuously. The various subscales of metacognitive skillfulness do not develop at the same pace; (2) Metacognitive skills have their own contribution to the prediction of learning performance, on top of intellectual ability. The relation between intellectual ability and metacognitive skills as predictors of learning performance is not affected by development between 12 and 15 yrs.; (3) Around the age of 15 yrs. metacognitive skills become fully general.LEI Universiteit LeidenDevelopmental pathways of social-emotional and cognitive functioning - ou

    The effects of facial attractiveness and trustworthiness in online peer-to-peer markets

    Get PDF
    Online peer-to-peer markets, such as Airbnb, often include profile photos of sellers to reduce anonymity. Ert, Fleischer, and Magen (2016) found that more trustworthy-looking, but not more attractive-looking, Airbnb hosts from Stockholm charge higher prices for similar apartments. This suggests that people are willing to pay more for a night in an apartment if the host looks trustworthy. Here, we present a pre-registered replication testing how photo-based impressions of hosts’ attractiveness and trustworthiness influence rental prices. We extend previous investigations by (a) controlling for additional features related to price (e.g., the apartment’s location value), (b) testing for an influence of other host features, such as race and facial expression, and (c) analyzing a substantially larger sample of apartments. An analysis of 1,020 listings in New York City showed that more attractive-looking, but not more trustworthy-looking, hosts charge higher prices for their apartments. Compared to White hosts, Black (but not Asian) hosts charge lower prices for their apartments. Hosts who smile more intensely in their profile photo charge higher prices. Our results support the general conclusion that people rely on profile photos in online markets, though we find that attractiveness is more important than trustworthiness. Keywords: first impressions, peer-to-peer markets, trustworthiness, attractivenes

    Response: commentary: distrust, false cues, and below-chance deception detection accuracy: commentary on Stel et al. (2020) and further reflections on (un)conscious lie detection from the perspective of truth-default theory

    Get PDF
    People often mistake other people’s deceits for truths (i.e., the truth bias; McCornack and Parks,1986). The Adaptive-Lie-Detector theory suggests that people make informed judgments usingreliable cues. A possible explanation for the truth bias is that when cues are absent, people makean “educated guess” based on most communication being honest (Street, 2015). Stel et al. (2020)investigated whether inducing contextual distrust could be the antidote for this bias. Based onprevious evidence that (1) distrust may induce conscious thought (e.g., Mayo, 2015) and (2)conscious processes can hinder the ability to detect deception (e.g., Reinhard et al., 2013), weexpected and found that participants are less accurate in judging deceits and truths when contextualdistrust (vs. trust) is induced, which was partly due to participants relying more on false beliefsabout deception.In his commentary on Stel et al. (2020), Levine (2021) agreed that (1) distrust hampers correctdeception judgments and that (2) distrust involves conscious processing. He was, however, skepticalthat deception cues could explain why distrust hampered truth detection. The main arguments werethat Stel et al. found (1) below chance-accuracy in the distrust condition, (2) which was explainedby more reliance on false deception cues. Levine states that the deception cues used in Stel et al.are generally non-diagnostic rather than antidiagnostic. He argued the findings are not in line withprevious findings and his theoretical perspective. Here, we react to these comments and argue thatour findings do not contradict, but expand previous findings.Social decision makin

    GRIP3 Brand Venlo-Blerick 7 augustus 2018

    Get PDF

    Whole-body glucose oxidation rate during prolonged exercise in type 1 diabetic patients under usual life conditions

    Get PDF
    Objective Fuel oxidation during exercise was studied in type 1 insulin-dependent (T1DM) patients mainly under quite constant insulin and glycemia; these protocols, however, likely do not reflect patients' usual metabolic conditions. The glucose oxidation rate (GLUox) in T1DM patients under usual life conditions was thus investigated during prolonged exercise (3-h) and its behavior was described mathematically. Materials/Methods Whole-body GLUox was determined in eight T1DM patients (4/8 M; aged 35-59 years) and eight well-matched healthy subjects. Venous blood was drawn prior to and every 30 min until the end of exercise; glycemia, insulin, cortisol, and growth hormone concentrations were determined. Oxygen consumption, carbon dioxide production, and ventilation were measured at rest and thereafter every 30 min of the exercise. To prevent hypoglycemia, patients were given fruit fudge (93% sucrose) prior to / during exercise. Results Insulin concentration and glycemia were significantly higher in patients across the entire exercise period (group effect, p < 0.001 for both). GLUox decreased significantly with increasing exercise duration (time effect, p < 0.001), but no significant difference was detected between the two groups (group effect, p = NS). GLUox, expressed as the percentage of the starting value, was described by an exponential function showing a time constant of 90 min (n = 96; mean corrected R2 = 0.666). Conclusions GLUox in T1DM patients was not significantly different from the rate observed in the control subjects. The function describing the time course of GLUox may be useful to correct an estimated GLUox for the duration of exercise and help T1DM patients avoiding exercise-induced glycemic imbalances
    • …
    corecore