25 research outputs found

    Syntax errors do not disrupt acoustic communication in the common cuckoo

    Get PDF
    When acoustic communication signals are distorted, receivers may misunderstand the signal, rendering it ineffective. Common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) are popularly known for the males’ simple, two-note advertisement calls, the “cu-coo” used for declaring the male’s breeding territories. Cuckoos do not learn their calls (vocal non-learners), so they are expected to have a limited ability to produce different acoustic signals. Nevertheless, male cuckoos appear to make syntax errors (e.g., repeated, reversed, or fragmented elements) even in their simple advertisement calls. We conducted a playback experiment with male cuckoos, broadcasting ten call types, including seven modified calls with errors (e.g. “cu-cu”, and “coo-cu”) and three natural calls used for comparisons (“cu-coo”, “cu-cu-coo”, and interspecific control). Male cuckoos responded in a manner suggesting that the presence of the first (“cu”) note of the natural 2-note “cu-coo” call in any form or combination yield effective signals. However, through the elevated frequency (by about 200 Hz) and greater speed of the “cu” note, the natural 3-note version “cu-cu-coo” call appears to have gained a novel communicative function in signalling with female cuckoos. Thus, syntax errors in calls with the “cu” element are not responsible for changing the function of the male cuckoos’ “cu-coo” call

    Male common cuckoos use a three-note variant of their "cu-coo" call for duetting with conspecific females.

    Get PDF
    Duetting is a coordinated form of acoustic communication with participants uttering calls or songs simultaneously and/or sequentially. Duetting is often observed in pair-bonded species, with mated females and males both contributing to the communal vocal output. We observed duetting between the sexes in the common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus), an obligate brood parasitic species without known pair formation. Specifically, female cuckoos use their sex-specific bubbling calls for duetting, while male cuckoos use a 3-note variant ("cu-cu-coo") of their typical and well-known 2-note ("cu-coo") territorial advertisement calls. The maximum frequency of the elements in the male's 3-note variants was higher relative to the 2-note calls, while durations of both the elements and the inter-element intervals were shorter. The vast majority (95 %) of the 3-note calling was detected together with the bubbling call, implying an intersexual duetting function, with the female calls preceding these male calls in 67 % of cases. The two call types in duetting followed each other rapidly (mean response time of females was 1.30 ± 0.71 SD s, and 0.76 ± 0.53 SD s in males), and typically overlapped with each other (95 %). Frequently (90 %), the male call was repeated 2-3 times, whereas the female call was repeated less frequently (9%). Our results are consistent with a main function of duetting in intersexual communication and coordination between female and male cuckoos

    Sex-specific responses to simulated territorial intrusions in the common cuckoo: a dual function of female acoustic signaling

    Get PDF
    The two-note call of the male common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus), the so-called “cu-coo”, is well known to people as a natural and cultural signal. However, the so-called “bubbling” call of the female cuckoo is almost unknown to most, and its function in the social organization of cuckoos remains understudied. We carried out a study of a possible intraspecific communication function of female bubbling calls, using playbacks to female cuckoos in their natural environment. Regarding vocal responses, both female and male cuckoos paid attention to the bubbling calls as they consistently responded acoustically by calling but did not so during control playbacks of collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto) calls. Accordingly, in about 63% of trials, females approached the loudspeaker closely and 81% uttered bubbling calls themselves during the experiment. These results are consistent with a function that the bubbling call plays a role in territorial signaling and defense among females. Male cuckoos also showed strong responses to playbacks of bubbling calls, as they approached the speaker and themselves called in 94% of playbacks; this is consistent with a scenario that they are interested in unfamiliar, new females in the area. Specifically, males approached the speaker repeatedly by flight, often flew around it and then perched on a tree, and uttered different call types beside the general “cu-coo” (e.g., quick “cu-cu-coo”, “gowk” call, and “guo” call). Our results represent an illustrative example that a simple female call may have multiple functions, as the cuckoo bubbling call advertises territory need for female cuckoos and attracts males

    NaĂŻve hosts of avian brood parasites accept foreign eggs, whereas older hosts fine-tune foreign egg discrimination during laying

    Get PDF
    BACKGROUND: Many potential hosts of social parasites recognize and reject foreign intruders, and reduce or altogether escape the negative impacts of parasitism. The ontogenetic basis of whether and how avian hosts recognize their own and the brood parasitic eggs remains unclear. By repeatedly parasitizing the same hosts with a consistent parasitic egg type, and contrasting the responses of naĂŻve and older breeders, we studied ontogenetic plasticity in the rejection of foreign eggs by the great reed warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus), a host species of the common cuckoo (Cuculus canorus). RESULTS: In response to experimental parasitism before the onset of laying, first time breeding hosts showed almost no egg ejection, compared to higher rates of ejection in older breeders. Young birds continued to accept foreign eggs when they were subjected to repeated parasitism, whereas older birds showed even higher ejection rates later in the same laying cycle. CONCLUSIONS: Our results are consistent with the hypotheses that (i) naĂŻve hosts need to see and learn the appearance of their own eggs to discriminate and reject foreign eggs, whereas (ii) experienced breeders possess a recognition template of their own eggs and reject parasitic eggs even without having to see their own eggs. However, we cannot exclude the possibility that other external cues and internal processes, accumulated simply with increasing age, may also modify age-specific patterns in egg rejection (e.g. more sightings of the cuckoo by older breeders). Future research should specifically track the potential role of learning in responses of individual hosts between first and subsequent breeding attempts by testing whether imprinting on a parasitized clutch reduces the rates of rejecting foreign eggs in subsequent parasitized clutches

    Call rate in Common Cuckoos does not predict body size and responses to conspecific playbacks

    Get PDF
    The brood parasitic Common Cuckoo Cuculus canorus is best known for its two-note “cu-coo” call which is almost continuously uttered by male during the breeding season and can be heard across long distances in the feld. Although the informative value of the cuckoo call was intensively investigated recently, it is still not clear whether call characteristic(s) indicate any of the phenotypic traits of the respective vocalising individuals. To fll this gap, we studied whether the call rate of male cuckoos (i.e., the number of calls uttered per unit of time) provides information on their body size, which might be a relevant trait during intrasexual territorial conficts. We captured free-living male cuckoos and measured their body size parameters (mass, wing, tail and tarsus lengths). Each subject was then radio-tagged, released, and its individual “cu-coo” calls were recorded soon after that in the feld. The results showed that none of the body size parameters covaried statistically with the call rates of individual male Common Cuckoos. In addition, we experimentally tested whether the “cu-coo” call rates afect behavioural responses of cuckoos using playbacks of either a quicker or a slower paced call than the calls with natural rates. Cuckoos responded similarly to both types of experimental playback treatments by approaching the speaker with statistically similar levels of responses as when presented with calls at the natural rate. We conclude that male Common Cuckoos do not advertise reliable information acoustically regarding their body size, and so, cuckoo calls are neither useful to characterize cuckoos’ phenotypic traits directly nor to indicate environmental quality indirectl

    Can common cuckoos discriminate between neighbours and strangers by their calls?

    Get PDF
    Common cuckoos (Cuculus canorus) are brood parasites: they lay their eggs in the nests of other bird species, and let manipulate these hosts into incubation their eggs and feed and rear the nestlings. Although cuckoos do not show parental care, they demonstrate complex social interactions, including territorial behaviours and male-to-male aggression. Cuckoos have a well-known and simple two-phase call ("cu" and "coo"), uttered by males during their breeding season. Previous studies suggested that the "cu-coo" call of males is individually unique, allowing discrimination between different classes of males. Using playback experiments in a dense population of radio-tagged cuckoos, we tested whether neighbouring males are tolerated more than unfamiliar intruders: the classic ”Dear Enemy” phenomenon. Focal birds responded more aggressively to the calls of unfamiliar simulated intruders (strangers) than to the calls of conspecifics with whom they shared territorial boundaries (familiar neighbours). Cuckoos responded quickly, within an average of less than half a minute, they often approached the loudspeaker to a proximity of less than 5 - 10 m, even from further distances (up to 80 m), and used their "cu-coo" calls in response. Our results reveal that cuckoos were able to use their simple call for the discrimination of familiar versus unfamiliar individuals, and they did so specifically to aggressively protect their own territories. In turn, cuckoos showed tolerance to nearby conspecifics, e.g., neighbours with overlapping territories and did not respond to control playbacks. Finally, as typically more than one cuckoo was interested in the playbacks, this study confirmed the opportunity for brood parasitic birds to socialize during the breeding season
    corecore