7 research outputs found

    The Effect of Age, Syntax Complexity, and Cognitive Ability on the Rate of Semantic Illusions

    Get PDF
    Semantic illusions are recognition errors that occur when an individual fails to notice that information contradicts their prior knowledge (Barton & Sanford, 1993; Erickson & Mattson, 1981). For example, after hearing the question, “If a plane crashes while flying over state lines, where should the survivors be buried?” many start to consider the legality or appropriateness of the scenario despite knowing “survivors” should not be buried. Having more knowledge does not necessarily prevent individuals from overlooking illusory information/misinformation. Older adults tend to have greater crystallized intelligence than young adults, yet these age groups appear to detect illusory information at equivalent rates (Umanath & Marsh, 2012; Umanath, 2014). However, there is also evidence that older adults experience more semantic illusions than young adults in general (Umanath et al., 2012). Previous research demonstrates that the rate of semantic illusions is sensitive to specific language structure manipulations, such as syntax structure or word placement that facilitate overlooking the illusory information (Bredart and Modolo, 1988; Büttner, 2007; Wang, Hagoort, & Yang, 2009). Furthermore, there is evidence that disrupting processing fluency by increasing the difficulty of reading enables more frequent detection of illusory information (Song, 2009). Although this effect has been demonstrated using easy- versus difficult-to-read font, increasing syntax complexity also increases reading difficulty and requires more effort for comprehension (e.g., Kemtes & Kemper, 1997; Stromswold et al., 1996). The current study used a combined experimental-correlational approach to investigate the effects of age, language structure, and cognitive ability on the rate of semantic illusions experienced in response to general knowledge questions. The experimental approach compared the rate of semantic illusions between young and older adult age groups for illusory information embedded in sentences with either simple or complex syntax structures. The correlational approach examined the best cognitive predictors of increased detection of illusory information among composite scores for crystallized intelligence, fluid intelligence, and rationality. The sample of 203 participants, including 114 young adults ( M = 24.98, SD = 4.06) and 89 older adults ( M = 65.63; SD = 4.93), was administered a semantic illusion task, general knowledge check, and reading comprehension task, along with a battery of cognitive measures assessing fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and rational thinking (Comprehensive Assessment of Rational Thinking [CART]; Stanovich, 2016). The semantic illusion task included general knowledge questions that either contained the correct information (target item), e.g., “How many animals of each kind did Noah bring on the ark?” or incorrect information (illusion item), e.g., How many animals of each kind did Moses bring on the ark?”. The sentence structure of the general knowledge questions varied across syntax complexity condition, such that participants experienced target items and illusion items in both simple (right-branching) versus complex (left-branching, middle-branching) syntax structures. Scoring procedures assessed frequencies for: (a) correct responses on target items (target score), (b) successful detection of illusory information (detection score), and (c) failures to detect illusory information (illusion score). The results of the experimental portion of the study confirmed an interaction of age and syntax for detection scores. Older adults detected illusory information more frequently than young adults, and complex versus simple syntax increased this advantage for the older adult age group. Alternatively, the pattern of results for illusion scores, or overlooking the illusory information, produced a main effect of age with older adults experiencing more semantic illusions than young adults regardless of syntax condition. Although counterintuitive, older adults had a higher baseline of prior knowledge, and therefore had more opportunities than young adults to detect and overlook the illusory information at higher rates. The correlational portion was largely data-driven, and investigated which cognitive composites for fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and rationality best predicted detection scores. Results demonstrated varying patterns between age groups, such that young adult detection scores were most accurately predicted by the rationality composite scores. However, older adult detection scores were best predicted by crystallized intelligence. Although both crystallized intelligence and rationality are positively associated with detection of illusory information (Hannon & Daneman, 2001; Mata et al., 2014), a mediation analysis revealed a potential underlying cause to the age-differences in the outcomes. A bootstrap mediation analysis indicated the effect of age group on detection scores was fully mediated by crystallized intelligence. More specifically, older adults had more prior knowledge than young adults to such a disparity, variation in detection scores between age groups can be fully accounted for by differences in crystallized intelligence between young and older adults. Overall, increased syntax complexity facilitates detection of illusory information compared to simple syntax. Furthermore, increased crystallized intelligence is associated with more frequent detection of illusory information. Yet, with less prior knowledge, performance on rational thinking problems is the better predictor of detecting illusory information

    Psychometric Network Analysis of the Hungarian WAIS

    Get PDF
    The positive manifold—the finding that cognitive ability measures demonstrate positive correlations with one another—has led to models of intelligence that include a general cognitive ability or general intelligence (g). This view has been reinforced using factor analysis and reflective, higher-order latent variable models. However, a new theory of intelligence, Process Overlap Theory (POT), posits that g is not a psychological attribute but an index of cognitive abilities that results from an interconnected network of cognitive processes. These competing theories of intelligence are compared using two different statistical modeling techniques: (a) latent variable modeling and (b) psychometric network analysis. Network models display partial correlations between pairs of observed variables that demonstrate direct relationships among observations. Secondary data analysis was conducted using the Hungarian Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition (H-WAIS-IV). The underlying structure of the H-WAIS-IV was first assessed using confirmatory factor analysis assuming a reflective, higher-order model and then reanalyzed using psychometric network analysis. The compatibility (or lack thereof) of these theoretical accounts of intelligence with the data are discussed

    The Relationship between Theory of Mind and Intelligence: A Formative <i>g</i> Approach

    No full text
    Theory of Mind (ToM) is the ability understand that other people’s mental states may be different from one’s own. Psychometric models have shown that individual differences in ToM can largely be attributed to general intelligence (g) (Coyle et al. 2018). Most psychometric models specify g as a reflective latent variable, which is interpreted as a general ability that plays a causal role in a broad range of cognitive tasks, including ToM tasks. However, an alternative approach is to specify g as a formative latent variable, that is, an overall index of cognitive ability that does not represent a psychological attribute (Kovacs and Conway 2016). Here we consider a formative g approach to the relationship between ToM and intelligence. First, we conducted an SEM with reflective g to test the hypothesis that ToM is largely accounted for by a general ability. Next, we conducted a model with formative g to determine whether the relationship between ToM and intelligence is influenced by domain-specific tasks. Finally, we conducted a redundancy analysis to examine the contribution of each g variable. Results suggest that the relationship between ToM and intelligence in this study was influenced by language-based tasks, rather than solely a general ability

    Chromosome Xq23 is associated with lower atherogenic lipid concentrations and favorable cardiometabolic indices

    No full text
    Abstract Autosomal genetic analyses of blood lipids have yielded key insights for coronary heart disease (CHD). However, X chromosome genetic variation is understudied for blood lipids in large sample sizes. We now analyze genetic and blood lipid data in a high-coverage whole X chromosome sequencing study of 65,322 multi-ancestry participants and perform replication among 456,893 European participants. Common alleles on chromosome Xq23 are strongly associated with reduced total cholesterol, LDL cholesterol, and triglycerides (min P = 8.5 × 10−72), with similar effects for males and females. Chromosome Xq23 lipid-lowering alleles are associated with reduced odds for CHD among 42,545 cases and 591,247 controls (P = 1.7 × 10−4), and reduced odds for diabetes mellitus type 2 among 54,095 cases and 573,885 controls (P = 1.4 × 10−5). Although we observe an association with increased BMI, waist-to-hip ratio adjusted for BMI is reduced, bioimpedance analyses indicate increased gluteofemoral fat, and abdominal MRI analyses indicate reduced visceral adiposity. Co-localization analyses strongly correlate increased CHRDL1 gene expression, particularly in adipose tissue, with reduced concentrations of blood lipids

    Erratum to: Guidelines for the use and interpretation of assays for monitoring autophagy (3rd edition) (Autophagy, 12, 1, 1-222, 10.1080/15548627.2015.1100356

    No full text
    non present

    Guidelines for the use and interpretation of assays for monitoring autophagy (3rd edition)

    No full text

    Chromosome Xq23 is associated with lower atherogenic lipid concentrations and favorable cardiometabolic indices

    No full text
    corecore