514 research outputs found

    Brief Report: Evidence of Ingroup Bias on the Shooter Task in a Saudi Sample

    Get PDF
    When predominantly White participants in Western countries are asked to shoot individuals in a computer game who may carry weapons, they show a greater bias to shoot at outgroup members and people stereotyped as dangerous. The goal was to determine the extent to which shooter biases in the Middle East would vary as a function of target ethnicity and culturally appropriate or inappropriate headgear. Within a sample of 37 male Saudi Arabian residents, we examined shooter biases outside of Western nations for the first time. Targets in this task were either White or Middle Eastern in appearance, and wore either American style baseball caps or a Saudi Arabian style shemagh and igal. Our results replicated the bias to shoot racial outgroup members observed in Western samples; we found a bias to shoot White over Middle Eastern targets. Unexpectedly, we also found a bias for Saudi participants to shoot at people wearing culturally appropriate traditional Saudi headgear over Western style baseball caps. To explain this latter finding, we cautiously speculate that relative perceptions of dangerousness in the Middle East may be influenced by media exposure and changing social conditions in the region

    Effects of Display Location Within Simulated Driving Environments

    Get PDF
    Driving simulators offer researchers experimental control while minimizing safety issues and reducing costs relative to on-road and test track experimental procedures. However, with the control of the visual environment that simulators allow, it can be tempting to develop experimental protocols that utilize displays within the visual environment of the simulator. Such displays have the potential to differentially affect driving performance based on their location within the driving environment. A simulator experiment was conducted in order to assess the effects of having drivers fixate a display at nine different locations on the center channel of a DriveSafety driving simulator. In general, driving performance was best when the display was in the middle of the screen. Both horizontal and vertical deviations from the center of the screen resulted in increased lane position variability, and drivers tended to drive closer to the opposite lane boundary toward which they were fixating when a display was located to the left or right of the center. In addition, response times to a task presented in the display were faster when the display was located toward the center of the screen

    Three geographically separate domestications of Asian rice

    Get PDF
    Domesticated rice (Oryza sativa L.) accompanied the dawn of Asian civilization(1) and has become one of world's staple crops. From archaeological and genetic evidence various contradictory scenarios for the origin of different varieties of cultivated rice have been proposed, the most recent based on a single domestication(2,3). By examining the footprints of selection in the genomes of different cultivated rice types, we show that there were three independent domestications in different parts of Asia. We identify wild populations in southern China and the Yangtze valley as the source of the japonica gene pool, and populations in Indochina and the Brahmaputra valley as the source of the indica gene pool. We reveal a hitherto unrecognized origin for the aus variety in central India or Bangladesh. We also conclude that aromatic rice is a result of a hybridization between japonica and aus, and that the tropical and temperate versions of japonica are later adaptations of one crop. Our conclusions are in accord with archaeological evidence that suggests widespread origins of rice cultivation(1,4). We therefore anticipate that our results will stimulate a more productive collaboration between genetic and archaeological studies of rice domestication, and guide utilization of genetic resources in breeding programmes aimed at crop improvement.European Research Council [339941]info:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersio

    Purified human factor VIII procoagulant protein: comparative hemostatic response after infusions into hemophilic and von Willebrand disease dogs.

    Get PDF
    The procoagulant protein F.VIII:C is noncovalently bound to von Willebrand factor (vWF) to give the factor VIII macromolecular complex. New highly purified preparations of isolated human F.VIII:C, devoid of vWF and about 500,000-fold purified, were administered to hemophilia A and von Willebrand disease (vWD) dogs to determine their hemostatic effectiveness and survival in the circulation. Two preparations of F.VIII:C were used: peak 1, with active components of Mr 185,000-280,000, and peak 2, with a single component of Mr 170,000. In hemophilic dogs, with no plasma F.VIII:C but normal vWF, both preparations immediately elevated plasma F.VIII:C to expected levels, promptly stopped induced and spontaneous hemorrhages, and gave sustained plasma levels of F.VIII:C. The isolated F.VIII:C immediately complexed with endogenous vWF in hemophilic plasma and was eliminated exponentially, with a half-life (t1/2) of about 9 hr. Survival of peak 2 F.VIII:C was longer than that of peak 1 material. In contrast, F.VIII:C complexed to vWF in a therapeutic concentrate administered to hemophilic dogs was eliminated biexponentially with first-phase t1/2 of 3.2 hr and second-phase t1/2 of 9 hr. In vWD dogs with no vWF and reduced F.VIII:C levels, the isolated F.VIII:C produced supernormal levels of F.VIII:C without effect on induced bleeding. It was rapidly eliminated from plasma with a t1/2 of about 1 hr, as was the complexed F.VIII:C in the concentrate. These data indicate that isolated F.VIII:C promptly complexes with vWF and in this form is highly effective in controlling hemophilic hemorrhages with good survival in plasma. Without endogenous vWF with which to complex, the F.VIII:C is promptly eliminated

    Games in Higher Education

    Get PDF
    International audienceThis entry presents an overview of how and why Learning Games are used in higher education.Learning Games can be defined as games that are designed to captivate the learners’ attention and facilitate their learning process. They have explicit educational purposes and can be used for teaching at all levels of education. All types of games can be used for learning: board games, card games, role-playing games, First Person Shooter games, simulation games, management games, puzzle games, treasure hunts…The main characteristic of Learning Games for higher education is the fact that they are designed to teach specific complex skills taught at university or during professional training programs. Unfortunately, it is not infrequent to observe strong opposition on the part of this target audience to this mode of learning, that these adult students associate with children.The use of Learning Games in primary school seems natural to teachers and is encouraged by specialists in didactics and neuroscience. This learning technique is much less frequently used in middle school and is almost completely absent from higher education. Yet teachers at all these levels are faced with the same problems, such as lack of motivation and investment, for which games are known to be an effective solution. This entry presents an overview of the games that can be used for higher education and the reasons why some teachers and students still show resistance to this type of learning. The numerous advantages of games for higher education will then be presented, citing games presently used in universities, in graduate schools and for professional training. Finally, thisDraft : Marfisi-Schottman I. (2019) Games in Higher Education. In: Tatnall A. (eds) Encyclopedia of Education and Information Technologies. Springer, Chamentry presents the current research questions that need to be addressed concerning the design of games for higher education and the acceptance of these games by teachers
    • …
    corecore