14 research outputs found

    Vision First? The Development of Primary Visual Cortical Networks Is More Rapid Than the Development of Primary Motor Networks in Humans

    Get PDF
    The development of cortical functions and the capacity of the mature brain to learn are largely determined by the establishment and maintenance of neocortical networks. Here we address the human development of long-range connectivity in primary visual and motor cortices, using well-established behavioral measures - a Contour Integration test and a Finger-tapping task - that have been shown to be related to these specific primary areas, and the long-range neural connectivity within those. Possible confounding factors, such as different task requirements (complexity, cognitive load) are eliminated by using these tasks in a learning paradigm. We find that there is a temporal lag between the developmental timing of primary sensory vs. motor areas with an advantage of visual development; we also confirm that human development is very slow in both cases, and that there is a retained capacity for practice induced plastic changes in adults. This pattern of results seems to point to human-specific development of the “canonical circuits” of primary sensory and motor cortices, probably reflecting the ecological requirements of human life

    Posterior–Anterior Brain Maturation Reflected in Perceptual, Motor and Cognitive Performance

    No full text
    Based on several postmortem morphometric and in vivo imaging studies it has been postulated that brain maturation roughly follows a caudal to rostral direction. In this study, we linked this maturational pattern to psychological function employing a series of well-established behavioral tasks. We addressed three distinct functions and brain regions with a perceptual (contour integration, CI), motor (finger tapping, FT), and executive control (Navon global–local) task. Our purpose was to investigate basic visual integration functions relying on primary visual cortex (V1) in CI; motor coordination function related to primary motor cortex (M1) in FT, and the executive control component, switching, related to the dorsolateral prefrontal region of the brain in the Navon task. 122 volunteer subjects were recruited to participate in this study between the ages of 10 and 20 (females n = 63, males n = 59). Employing conventional statistical methods, we found that 10 and 12 year olds are performing significantly weaker than 20 year olds in all three tasks. In the CI and Navon global–local tasks, even 14 years old perform poorer than adults. We have also investigated the developmental trajectories by fitting sigmoid curves on our data streams. The analysis of the developmental trajectories of the three tasks showed a posterior to anterior pattern in the emergence of the developmental functions with the earliest development in the visual CI task (V1), followed by motor development in the FT task (M1), and cognitive development as measured in the Navon global–local task (DLPC) being the slowest. Gender difference was also present in FT task showing an earlier maturation for girls in the motor domain

    Mapping hand function in rare neurodevelopmental disorders

    Get PDF
    Introduction: Appropriate level of hand function is a key to participation in daily living activities, education and social life. Mapping functional abilities related to hand may be challenging but important for ensuring participation in rare neurodevelopmental disorders. We previously showed specific patterns of motor control and learning capacity of the hand in Williams syndrome, a genetically originated neurodevelopmental disorder that involves intellectual disability and motor deficits. Aim: Our aim in the present study was to further map the functional motor skills related to daily living activities and possible sensory dysfunction related to the hand in this rare neurodevelopmental disorder. Method: Participation in activities related to hand function was assessed by the Jebsen-Taylor Hand Function Test. Maximum motor speed in terms of index finger tapping and somatosensory function in terms of two-point discrimination and position sense were assessed. Results and conclusion: Descriptive data analysis revealed that participation in the daily living activities shows difficulties for individuals with Williams syndrome in all domains. Moreover, somatosensory deficits and limitations in motor speed may accompany functional challenges. We also found that the Jebsen-Taylor Hand Function Test was appropriate to use and is a promising tool for daily living activity assessment in the case of mild and moderate intellectual disability with the exception of the “writing” subtest. Regarding somatosensory testing, two-point discrimination test was not applicable for all participants position sense. Our results support the need for further establishment of the relationship between neurophysiological, sensory and motor functional characteristics related to hand

    Age groups of participants in the CI and FT tasks.

    No full text
    <p>Age groups of participants in the CI and FT tasks.</p

    Developmental-learning surfaces.

    No full text
    <p>(A) Developmental-learning surface in CI. Performance threshold of each age-group is expressed in degrees of orientation jitter along the contour as a function of age and practice. Performance in CI increases as a function of age, suggesting that contour integration has a slow developmental course. Performance also increases as a function of practice, with a faster progression of learning in the younger age-groups at the beginning of practice. However, in the later phases of training, all age-groups learn at the same rate. (B) Developmental-learning surface in FT. Performance rate (number of taps/second) is expressed as a function of age and practice. Performance in FT increases both as a function of age and practice, similarly to CI.</p

    Comparison of learning rates in Contour Integration and Finger-tapping.

    No full text
    <p>Day 1 performance is considered 100%, and performance in subsequent days is expressed relative to that. Improvements are calculated by taking the difference between thresholds in consecutive days of practice (such as, Day 1–Day 2, Day 2–Day 3, Day 3–Day 4, Day 4–Day 5). There is a larger improvement from Day 1 to Day 2 in FT than in CI across all age-groups. This difference vanishes from Day 2 to Day 3, and learning rates become nearly equivalent in the two tasks after Day 3.</p

    Summary of the methods and results.

    No full text
    <p>(A) Sideview of the human brain with the primary visual cortex (V1, Br 17) in blue, and the primary motor cortex (M1 or Br 4) in red. The cerebral cortex is generally divided into six functionally distinct layers, and the principal source of long-range lateral intralaminar connections is layer II and III, as shown in the insets corresponding to V1 and M1. (B) Contour Integration (CI) stimuli, addressing long-range connections in the primary visual cortex. The collinear chain of oriented elements forming a horizontally placed egg-shape is hidden in the background of randomly positioned and oriented elements. The panels show three levels of difficulty in the CI task. Practice and development leads to improved performance. (C) Movement-sequence in the Finger-tapping (FT) task addressing long-range connectivity of the primary motor cortex. Accuracy and speed of carrying out this sequence improves following practice and during the course of development. (D) Developmental curves in CI (blue) and in FT (red). Day 2 performance of each age-group was normalized to that of the adult performance in each task. Small symbols: individual data; large symbols: age-group average. Curve fitting was done on the age-group average values. The horizontal lines at the bottom connect two age-groups (15 and 21 y), and significance levels of the difference in performance in the two tasks, respectively, are denoted.</p
    corecore