25 research outputs found
Taphonomic Criteria for Identifying Iberian Lynx Dens in Quaternary Deposits
For decades, taphonomists have dedicated their efforts to assessing the nature of the massive leporid accumulations recovered at archaeological sites in the northwestern Mediterranean region. Their interest lying in the fact that the European rabbit constituted a critical part of human subsistence during the late Pleistocene and early Holocene. However, rabbits are also a key prey in the food webs of Mediterranean ecosystems and the base of the diet for several specialist predators, including the Iberian lynx (Lynx pardinus). For this reason, the origin of rabbit accumulations in northwestern Mediterranean sites has proved a veritable conundrum. Here, we present the zooarchaeological and taphonomic study of more than 3000 faunal and 140 coprolite remains recovered in layer IIIa of Cova del Gegant (Catalonia, Spain). Our analysis indicates that this layer served primarily as a den for the Iberian lynx. The lynxes modified and accumulated rabbit remains and also died at the site creating an accumulation dominated by the two taxa. However, other agents and processes, including human, intervened in the final configuration of the assemblage. Our study contributes to characterizing the Iberian lynx fossil accumulation differentiating between the faunal assemblages accumulated by lynxes and hominins
Effect of survey instrument on participation in a follow-up study: a randomization study of a mailed questionnaire versus a computer-assisted telephone interview
<p>Abstract</p> <p>Background</p> <p>Many epidemiological and public health surveys report increasing difficulty obtaining high participation rates. We conducted a pilot follow-up study to determine whether a mailed or telephone survey would better facilitate data collection in a subset of respondents to an earlier telephone survey conducted as part of the National Birth Defects Prevention Study.</p> <p>Methods</p> <p>We randomly assigned 392 eligible mothers to receive a self-administered, mailed questionnaire (MQ) or a computer-assisted telephone interview (CATI) using similar recruitment protocols. If mothers gave permission to contact the fathers, fathers were recruited to complete the same instrument (MQ or CATI) as mothers.</p> <p>Results</p> <p>Mothers contacted for the MQ, within all demographic strata examined, were more likely to participate than those contacted for the CATI (86.6% vs. 70.6%). The median response time for mothers completing the MQ was 17âdays, compared to 29âdays for mothers completing the CATI. Mothers completing the MQ also required fewer reminder calls or letters to finish participation versus those assigned to the CATI (median 3 versus 6), though they were less likely to give permission to contact the father (75.0% vs. 85.8%). Fathers contacted for the MQ, however, had higher participation compared to fathers contacted for the CATI (85.2% vs. 54.5%). Fathers recruited to the MQ also had a shorter response time (median 17âdays) and required fewer reminder calls and letters (median 3 reminders) than those completing the CATI (medians 28âdays and 6 reminders).</p> <p>Conclusions</p> <p>We concluded that offering a MQ substantially improved participation rates and reduced recruitment effort compared to a CATI in this study. While a CATI has the advantage of being able to clarify answers to complex questions or eligibility requirements, our experience suggests that a MQ might be a good survey option for some studies.</p
Learning by Heart: Cultural Patterns in the Faunal Processing Sequence during the Middle Pleistocene
Social learning, as an information acquisition process, enables intergenerational transmission and the stabilisation of cultural
forms, generating and sustaining behavioural traditions within human groups. Archaeologically, such social processes might
become observable by identifying repetitions in the record that result from the execution of standardised actions. From
a zooarchaeological perspective, the processing and consumption of carcasses may be used to identify these types of
phenomena at the sites. To investigate this idea, several faunal assemblages from Bolomor Cave (Valencia, Spain, MIS 9-5e)
and Gran Dolina TD10-1 (Burgos, Spain, MIS 9) were analysed. The data show that some butchery activities exhibit variability
as a result of multiple conditioning factors and, therefore, the identification of cultural patterns through the resulting cutmarks
presents additional difficulties. However, other activities, such as marrow removal by means of intentional breakage,
seem to reflect standardised actions unrelated to the physical characteristics of the bones. The statistical tests we applied
show no correlation between the less dense areas of the bones and the location of impacts. Comparison of our
experimental series with the archaeological samples indicates a counter-intuitive selection of the preferred locus of impact,
especially marked in the case of Bolomor IV. This fact supports the view that bone breakage was executed counterintuitively
and repetitively on specific sections because it may have been part of an acquired behavioural repertoire. These
reiterations differ between levels and sites, suggesting the possible existence of cultural identities or behavioural
predispositions dependant on groups. On this basis, the study of patterns could significantly contribute to the identification
of occupational strategies and organisation of the hominids in a territory. In this study, we use faunal data in identifying the
mechanics of intergenerational information transmission within Middle Pleistocene human communities and provide new
ideas for the investigation of occupational dynamics from a zooarchaeological approach