1,032 research outputs found

    From eye to machine: shifting authority in color measurement

    Get PDF
    Given a subject so imbued with contention and conflicting theoretical stances, it is remarkable that automated instruments ever came to replace the human eye as sensitive arbiters of color specification. Yet, dramatic shifts in assumptions and practice did occur in the first half of the twentieth century. How and why was confidence transferred from careful observers to mechanized devices when the property being measured – color – had become so closely identified with human physiology and psychology? A fertile perspective on the problem is via the history of science and technology, paying particular attention to social groups and disciplinary identity to determine how those factors affected their communities’ cognitive territory. There were both common and discordant threads motivating the various technical groups that took on the problems of measuring light and color from the late nineteenth century onwards, and leading them towards the development of appropriate instruments for themselves. The transition from visual to photoelectric methods <i>could</i> be portrayed as a natural evolution, replacing the eye by an alternative roviding more sensitivity and convenience – indeed, this is the conventional positivist view propounded by technical histories. However, the adoption of new measurement technologies seldom is simple, and frequently has a significant cultural component. Beneath this slide towards automation lay a raft of implicit assumptions about objectivity, the nature of the observer, the role of instruments, and the trade-offs between standardization and descriptive power. While espousing rational arguments for a physical detector of color, its proponents weighted their views with tacit considerations. The reassignment of trust from the eye to automated instruments was influenced as much by the historical context as by intellectual factors. I will argue that several distinct aspects were involved, which include the reductive view of color provided by the trichromatic theory; the impetus provided by its association with photometry; the expanding mood for a quantitative and objective approach to scientific observation; and, the pressures for commercial standardization. As suggested by these factors, there was another shift of authority at play: from one technical specialism to another. The regularization of color involved appropriation of the subject by a particular set of social interests: communities of physicists and engineers espousing a ‘physicalist’ interpretation, rather than psychologists and physiologists for whom color was conceived as a more complex phenomenon. Moreover, the sources for automated color measurement, and instrumentation for measuring color, were primarily from the industrial sphere rather than from academic science. To understand these shifts, then, this chapter explores differing views of the importance of observers, machines and automation

    Modern displays: Why we see different colors, and what it means?

    Get PDF
    International audienceOne of the basic tenets of conventional applied colorimetry is that the whole population of color normal observers can be represented by a single "standard" observer with reasonable accuracy. The 1964 CIE standard colorimetric observer has indeed served us well in all industrial color imaging applications, until recently. With the proliferation of modern wide-gamut displays with narrow-band primaries, color scientists and engineers face a new challenge. Various recent studies, including those by the current authors, have shown that the color perception on such displays varies significantly among color normal observers. Conventional colorimetry has no means to predict this variation. In this paper, we explore this problem by summarizing the results from an ongoing study, and explain the practical significance of this issue in the context of display applications

    Study of Observer Variability in Modern Display Colorimetry: Comparison of CIE 2006 Model and 10° Standard Observer

    Get PDF
    International audienceThis paper compares CIE 2006 model predictions and the 1964 10° standard colorimetric observer with the average observer data from three distinct subgroups of 47 Stiles-Burch observers formed on the basis of observer ages. For two of these subgroups, the long-wave sensitive (x-) color matching functions obtained from the CIE06 model did not accurately predict the intra-group average observer functions. In terms of display color perception, the prediction error is reflected in the longer wavelengths of the color spectrum. In contrast, the short-wave sensitive (z-) function of the 10° standard observer showed significant deviations from the intra-group average observer for all three subgroups

    Electrochromic properties of a poly(dithienylfuran) derivative featuring a redox-active dithiin unit

    Get PDF
    A teraryl monomer containing a 1,4-dithiin-furan central unit has been synthesised and characterised by single crystal X-ray crystallography. The di(thienyl)furan monomer 11 was successfully polymerised electrochemically and shown to possess a lower electrochemical band gap than its terthiophene analogue (1.97 eV cf. 2.11 eV). The electrochromic properties of this polymer proved to be superior to PEDOT, with fast switching and reversible colour transformation at high colour contrast (CE = 212 cm(2) C-1 cf. 183 cm(2) C-1 for PEDOT at 95% optical switch)

    A new algorithm for calculating perceived colour difference of images

    Get PDF
    Faithful colour reproduction of digital images requires a reliable measure to compare such images in order to evaluate the reproduction performance. The conventional methods attempt to apply the CIE Colorimetry based colour difference equations, such as CIELAB, CMC, CIE94 and CIEDE2000, to complex images on a pixel-by-pixel basis, and calculates the overall colour difference as the averaged difference of each pixel in the image. This method is simple and straightforward but often does not represent the colour difference perceived by human visual system. This paper proposes a new algorithm for calculating the overall colour difference between a reproduced image and its original. The results obtained show that this new metric provides a quantitative measure that more closely corresponds to the colour difference perceived by human visual system

    Aesthetics appreciation of wood colour and patterns by colorimetry. Part 1. Colorimetry theory for the cielab system

    Get PDF
    The colorimetry theory for the CIELab system today allows us to compute the chromatic coordinates of the raw timber colour along the wood - chain industry. The wood industry is nd strongly behind in such areas. Progress in the wood appearance description by means of colour and pattern characteristics would be suitable to classify the wood. It would also help to match pieces of wood in furniture and inside the houses, to study the wood colour change in wood ageing by photodecoloration or by drying operations, and to improve wood colour variability

    In situ spectroelectrochemistry and colour measurement of a complementary electrochromic device based on surface-confined Prussian blue and aqueous solution-phase methyl viologen

    Get PDF
    The fabrication, in situ spectroelectrochemistry and colour measurement of hybrid electrochromic devices (ECDs) based on a surface-confined metal hexacyanometallate – Prussian blue (PB, containing the iron(III) hexacyanoferrate(II) chromophore) – and aqueous solution-phase methyl viologen (N,N´-dimethyl-4,4´-bipyridylium) are described. In the ECDs, the initial (‘off’) bleached state is set with PB in its reduced form and the methyl viologen as the di-cation. Switching to the coloured state (‘on’), forms the mixed-valence iron(III) hexacyanoferrate(II) chromophore on oxidation of iron(II) hexacyanoferrate(II), with simultaneous reduction of the methyl viologen dication to form a mixture of the radical cation monomer/dimer. Using the Commission Internationale de l'Eclairage (CIE) system of colorimetry, the colour stimulus of such ECDs and the changes that take place on reversibly switching between the colourless and coloured states have been calculated from in situ visible region spectra recorded under electrochemical control. The concentration of the solution-phase methyl viologen and its diffusion to the cathode controlled both the proportion of surface-confined (reduced) PB that is switched to the blue form and the overall ECD changes. For the ECDs’ ‘on’ states, the CIELAB 1976 color space coordinates for a D55 illuminant were L* = 60, a* = 3 and b* = −46, and L* = 49, a* = 9 and b* = −59, respectively for 5 and 10 mM methyl viologen solution concentrations. The low a* and high (negative) b* chromaticity coordinates quantified the overall ECD colour stimulus of the ‘on’ state as being deep blue, with a broad absorption across the visible spectral region. Combination of the methyl viologen system in the ECDs served to remove the green tint perceived in single film PB. CIELAB 1976 colour space coordinates showed that the ECDs were fully transparent and nearly colourless in the ‘off’ states, with L* = 100, a* = 1 and b* = 1. The changes in the transparency were 83.0% (5 mM methyl viologen) and 93.1% (10 mM methyl viologen) between the ‘off’ (bleached) and ‘on’ (coloured) states of the ECDs

    Computer mediated colour fidelity and communication

    Get PDF
    Developments in technology have meant that computercontrolled imaging devices are becoming more powerful and more affordable. Despite their increasing prevalence, computer-aided design and desktop publishing software has failed to keep pace, leading to disappointing colour reproduction across different devices. Although there has been a recent drive to incorporate colour management functionality into modern computer systems, in general this is limited in scope and fails to properly consider the way in which colours are perceived. Furthermore, differences in viewing conditions or representation severely impede the communication of colour between groups of users. The approach proposed here is to provide WYSIWYG colour across a range of imaging devices through a combination of existing device characterisation and colour appearance modeling techniques. In addition, to further facilitate colour communication, various common colour notation systems are defined by a series of mathematical mappings. This enables both the implementation of computer-based colour atlases (which have a number of practical advantages over physical specifiers) and also the interrelation of colour represented in hitherto incompatible notations. Together with the proposed solution, details are given of a computer system which has been implemented. The system was used by textile designers for a real task. Prior to undertaking this work, designers were interviewed in order to ascertain where colour played an important role in their work and where it was found to be a problem. A summary of the findings of these interviews together with a survey of existing approaches to the problems of colour fidelity and communication in colour computer systems are also given. As background to this work, the topics of colour science and colour imaging are introduced

    Achromatic perception in color image displays

    Get PDF
    The perception of achromatic colors is an important aspect of CRT color appearance. Achromatic colors are important for practical reasons such as image color balance and as fundamental components of chromatic adaptation and color constancy research. Data on achromatic colors is absolutely essential for applying CIE colorimetry to CRT-hardcopy matching. For example, the CIELAB formulas require the specification of the tristimulus values of white. Unfortunately, psychophysical data on the perception of achromatic colors, including white, in CRT-hardcopy matching situations is not readily available in the literature. The purpose of this research was to investigate factors that affect the perception of achromatic colors in CRT and hardcopy images viewed in a desktop environment. Four psychophysical experiments were performed in this research. In these experiments, three observers made judgements of achromatic colors on a CRT monitor and in hardcopy images in isolated and matching situations. The color of image balance and ambient illumination in the laboratory was varied between 2700K tungsten and 6000K daylight-fluorescent. The results show that chromatic adaptation was controlled almost totally by the CRT image. Adaptation to tungsten was found to be incomplete. When adaptation was incomplete, the chromaticities of achromatic color judgements fell into two categories similar to Bartleson\u27s type I and II, where type II show higher color constancy. Judgements were more likely to be of type II when surface color attributes were present in the CRT image and when the observers were instructed to consider the CRT colors in surface mode. When the images contained more illuminant attributes, the results resembled type I. Hardcopy always produced type II results
    • …
    corecore