Abstract

[EN] The ability of virtual reality (VR) to recreate controlled, immersive, and interactive environments that provide intensive and customized exercises has motivated its therapeutic use after stroke. Interaction and bodily presence in VR-based interventions is usually mediated through virtual selves, which synchronously represent body movements or responses to events on external input devices. Embodied self-representations in the virtual world not only provide an anchor for visuomotor tasks, but their morphologies can have behavioral implications. While research has focused on the underlying subjective mechanisms of exposure to VR on healthy individuals, the transference of these findings to individuals with stroke is not evident and remains unexplored, which could affect the experience and, ultimately, the clinical effectiveness of neurorehabilitation interventions. This study determined and compared the sense of embodiment and presence elicited by a virtual environment under different perspectives and levels of immersion in healthy subjects and individuals with stroke. Forty-six healthy subjects and 32 individuals with stroke embodied a gender-matched neutral avatar in a virtual environment that was displayed in a first-person perspective with a head-mounted display and in a third-person perspective with a screen, and the participants were asked to interact in a virtual task for 10 min under each condition in counterbalanced order, and to complete two questionnaires about the sense of embodiment and presence experienced during the interaction. The sense of body-ownership, self-location, and presence were more vividly experienced in a first-person than in a third-person perspective by both healthy subjects (p < 0.001, eta(2)(p) = 0.212; p = 0.005, eta(2)(p) = 0.101; p = 0.001, eta(2)(p) = 0.401, respectively) and individuals with stroke (p = 0.019, eta(2)(p) = 0.070; p = 0.001, eta(2)(p) = 0.135; p = 0.014, eta(2)(p) = 0.077, respectively). In contrast, no agency perspective-related differences were found in any group. All measures were consistently higher for healthy controls than for individuals with stroke, but differences between groups only reached statistical significance in presence under the first-person condition (p < 0.010, eta(2)(p) = 0.084). In spite of these differences, the participants experienced a vivid sense of embodiment and presence in almost all conditions. These results provide first evidence that, although less intensively, embodiment and presence are similarly experienced by individuals who have suffered a stroke and by healthy individuals, which could support the vividness of their experience and, consequently, the effectiveness of VR-based interventions.This study was funded by Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad of Spain (Project RTC-2017-6051-7 and Grant BES-2014-068218), Fundació la Marató de la TV3 (Grant 201701-10), and Universitat Politècnica de València (Grant PAID-10-18). We acknowledge the support of NVIDIA Corporation with the donation of the Titan Xp GPU used for this research.Borrego, A.; Latorre, J.; Alcañiz Raya, ML.; Llorens Rodríguez, R. (2019). Embodiment and Presence in Virtual Reality After Stroke. A Comparative Study With Healthy Subjects. Frontiers in Neurology. 10:1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2019.01061S1810Berlucchi, G., & Aglioti, S. (1997). The body in the brain: neural bases of corporeal awareness. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(12), 560-564. doi:10.1016/s0166-2236(97)01136-3Legrand, D. (2006). The Bodily Self: The Sensori-Motor Roots of Pre-Reflective Self-Consciousness. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 5(1), 89-118. doi:10.1007/s11097-005-9015-6Arzy, S., Overney, L. S., Landis, T., & Blanke, O. (2006). Neural Mechanisms of Embodiment. Archives of Neurology, 63(7), 1022. doi:10.1001/archneur.63.7.1022De Vignemont, F. (2011). Embodiment, ownership and disownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(1), 82-93. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.004Giummarra, M. J., Gibson, S. J., Georgiou-Karistianis, N., & Bradshaw, J. L. (2008). Mechanisms underlying embodiment, disembodiment and loss of embodiment. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(1), 143-160. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.07.001Ma, K., & Hommel, B. (2015). The role of agency for perceived ownership in the virtual hand illusion. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 277-288. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.008Kilteni, K., Maselli, A., Kording, K. P., & Slater, M. (2015). Over my fake body: body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00141Clark, A., Kiverstein, J., & Vierkant, T. (Eds.). (2013). Decomposing the Will. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199746996.001.0001Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S.-J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 355(1404), 1771-1788. doi:10.1098/rstb.2000.0734Bermúdez i Badia, S., Fluet, G. G., Llorens, R., & Deutsch, J. E. (2016). Virtual Reality for Sensorimotor Rehabilitation Post Stroke: Design Principles and Evidence. Neurorehabilitation Technology, 573-603. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-28603-7_28Perez-Marcos, D., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., & Slater, M. (2011). Is my hand connected to my body? The impact of body continuity and arm alignment on the virtual hand illusion. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 6(4), 295-305. doi:10.1007/s11571-011-9178-5IJsselsteijn, W. A., de Kort, Y. A. W., & Haans, A. (2006). Is This My Hand I See Before Me? The Rubber Hand Illusion in Reality, Virtual Reality, and Mixed Reality. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 15(4), 455-464. doi:10.1162/pres.15.4.455Banakou, D., Groten, R., & Slater, M. (2013). Illusory ownership of a virtual child body causes overestimation of object sizes and implicit attitude changes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(31), 12846-12851. doi:10.1073/pnas.1306779110Yee, N., & Bailenson, J. (2007). The Proteus Effect: The Effect of Transformed Self-Representation on Behavior. Human Communication Research, 33(3), 271-290. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00299.xSteed, A., Frlston, S., Lopez, M. M., Drummond, J., Pan, Y., & Swapp, D. (2016). An ‘In the Wild’ Experiment on Presence and Embodiment using Consumer Virtual Reality Equipment. IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 22(4), 1406-1414. doi:10.1109/tvcg.2016.2518135Colomer, C., Llorens, R., Noé, E., & Alcañiz, M. (2016). Effect of a mixed reality-based intervention on arm, hand, and finger function on chronic stroke. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 13(1). doi:10.1186/s12984-016-0153-6Laver, K. E., Lange, B., George, S., Deutsch, J. E., Saposnik, G., & Crotty, M. (2017). Virtual reality for stroke rehabilitation. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. doi:10.1002/14651858.cd008349.pub4Llorens, R., Borrego, A., Palomo, P., Cebolla, A., Noé, E., i Badia, S. B., & Baños, R. (2017). Body schema plasticity after stroke: Subjective and neurophysiological correlates of the rubber hand illusion. Neuropsychologia, 96, 61-69. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.01.007Zeller, D., Gross, C., Bartsch, A., Johansen-Berg, H., & Classen, J. (2011). Ventral Premotor Cortex May Be Required for Dynamic Changes in the Feeling of Limb Ownership: A Lesion Study. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(13), 4852-4857. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5154-10.2011Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). «Mini-mental state». Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189-198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6Romero, M., Sánchez, A., Marín, C., Navarro, M. D., Ferri, J., & Noé, E. (2012). Clinical usefulness of the Spanish version of the Mississippi Aphasia Screening Test (MASTsp): validation in stroke patients. Neurología (English Edition), 27(4), 216-224. doi:10.1016/j.nrleng.2011.06.001Latorre, J., Llorens, R., Colomer, C., & Alcañiz, M. (2018). Reliability and comparison of Kinect-based methods for estimating spatiotemporal gait parameters of healthy and post-stroke individuals. Journal of Biomechanics, 72, 268-273. doi:10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.03.008Lloréns, R., Noé, E., Naranjo, V., Borrego, A., Latorre, J., & Alcañiz, M. (2015). Tracking Systems for Virtual Rehabilitation: Objective Performance vs. Subjective Experience. A Practical Scenario. Sensors, 15(3), 6586-6606. doi:10.3390/s150306586Slater, M., & Steed, A. (2000). A Virtual Presence Counter. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 9(5), 413-434. doi:10.1162/105474600566925Slater, M., Spanlang, B., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., & Blanke, O. (2010). First Person Experience of Body Transfer in Virtual Reality. PLoS ONE, 5(5), e10564. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010564Petkova, V. I., Khoshnevis, M., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). The Perspective Matters! Multisensory Integration in Ego-Centric Reference Frames Determines Full-Body Ownership. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00035Maselli, A., & Slater, M. (2013). The building blocks of the full body ownership illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00083Debarba, H. G., Molla, E., Herbelin, B., & Boulic, R. (2015). Characterizing embodied interaction in First and Third Person Perspective viewpoints. 2015 IEEE Symposium on 3D User Interfaces (3DUI). doi:10.1109/3dui.2015.7131728Burin, D., Livelli, A., Garbarini, F., Fossataro, C., Folegatti, A., Gindri, P., & Pia, L. (2015). Are Movements Necessary for the Sense of Body Ownership? Evidence from the Rubber Hand Illusion in Pure Hemiplegic Patients. PLOS ONE, 10(3), e0117155. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0117155Post-stroke cognitive disorders TeasellR SalterK FaltynekP CotoiA EskesG Evidence-Based Review of Stroke Rehabilitatio

    Similar works

    Full text

    thumbnail-image

    Available Versions

    Last time updated on 03/11/2020