149 research outputs found

    Volatile organic compounds in commonly used beddings before and after autoclaving

    Get PDF
    How to describe bedding, that is the question. So far it has been acceptable to write down the type and manufacturer of the bedding. And maybe for quality purposes screen the bedding for pesticide and hcav y metal residues. This study focused on assessing of ten volatile organic compounds, which as a group has been combined with negative effects on animals. The commonly used European beddings were found to contain tremendously variable concentrations of these arbitrarily chosen volatile compounds. Furthermore, in most cases the concentrations went down by an order of magnitude after autoclaving. In conclusion, description of bedding in sensitive studies remains vague unless there is data on volatile organic compounds of the bedding at its final destination, the animal cage

    Improved method for vaginal plug detection in rats

    Get PDF
    Time mating of rodents is essential in many experimental procedures. The presence of the vaginal plug is frequently used as an indicator to confirm that mating has occurred. In rats, vaginal plugs are often deep and therefore difficult to detect byvulvar inspection. This study was carried out to assess the applicability of a human otoscope for detection of deep plugs, and the predictive value of plugs for pregnancy. A total of 132 Wistar female rats in proestrous were mated and examinedfor vaginal plugs on the next morning with an otoscope. All the plugs were located deep inside the vagina and could not be seen without the scope. The pregnancy rate of all females in proestrous was 69.7 %. Of all females, 72.7% had a vaginal plug and the positive predictive value of plug detection was 91.7 %. The sensitivity of the vaginal plug method was 95.7 % and specificity 80.0%. In conclusion, this study shows that detection of plugs has value for predicting pregnancies in rats. The method devised is quick, straightforward and painless to the animals and it is easily taught to technicians

    Deciduous wood chips as bedding material: Estimation of dust yield, water absorption and microbiological comparison

    Get PDF
    Dust yield, water absorbing capacity and airborne microbes of laboratory animal beddings made of deciduous wood were compared. Three different bedding materials, aspen, alder and birch, two chip sizes and effects of a softening treatment were studied. Overall dust yield from Chips was relatively low. Aspen should, however, be preferred to alder, because it was less dusty. Small (1 X 2 X 1 mm) aspen Chips yielded less inhalable dust than the large chips (4 X 4 X 1 mm). Water absorbing capacity was better in small chips than in large chips and could also be increased with a softening procedure. In this study softening of small aspen chips resulted in unacceptable high dust yield. Though alder had a better water absorbing capacity, aspen seems to soak up urine well enough during a normal change cycle

    Serum antibody response to canine distemper Virus vaccines in beagle dogs

    Get PDF
    The three commercial triple CDV vaccines used for vaccination differ by the Virus strain and the method of production. The immunogenicity of these vaccines was studied in beagle dogs under experimental conditions using neutralizing antibodytiters as the measure.Differences in antibody responses were detected between the three distemper vaccines. Rockborn strain produced in canine cells (vaccine 1) induced on average the highest titers and smallest variance of serum neutra1i7ing antibodies betweenvaccine groups. There was also a significant difference between the 2 Onderstepoort strains (vaccine 2 and 3) inducing CDV antibodies in dogs one month after first vaccination indicating difference in antigenicity between the vaccines

    Hybridityö ja -oppiminen pandemian jälkeen

    Get PDF

    The use of aspen blocks and tubes to enrich the cage environment of laboratory rats

    Get PDF
    Female and male outbred Wistar rats (n=48) were allocated into three groups at weaning; control, tube and block groups (n = 8 males and 8 females in each) Animals were conventionally housed for five weeks in groups of four per cage with either an aspen tube (20x12x12 cm) or an aspen block (6x6x6 cm) in addition to the aspen bedding. The control animals had no enrichment items in their cages. The use of tube and block was assessed by measuring both the volume gnawed and via video recordings, which were done by the instantaneous sampling method at 1 min intervals. The growth of the animals was followed by weighing the animals three times during the study. Rats with the tubes in their cage spent over, 80 % of their time during light period inside the tube and over 20 % during dark period. Furthermore, when the lights were turned off, rats increased other contacts (on and beside) with the tube from 5 % to about 40 %. Animals with blocks spent about 3 % of their time during the light period on top of the block or in its vicinity and about 11 % during the dark period. The amount gnawed was essentially the same with both items and increased slightly with time. The enrichment items did not have any effects on the growth of the animals. In conclusion, the enrichment items have the advantage of not introducing any extra or new compounds into the cage environment, since they are made from the same raw material as the bedding. The rats used the items not only for gnawing but also for other activities. The inside of the tube was mainly used as shelter from the light The aspen tube seemed to have more enrichment value for rats than the aspen block‘ since it allowed a wider range of behaviour patterns to be expressed. These items were reusable, economical and species-appropriate for en riching the cage environment of laboratory rats. Keywords: environmental enrichment, gnawing, rat

    Work for Food – A Solution to Restricting Food Intake in Group Housed Rats?

    Get PDF
    Rodents spend a great proportion of their time searching for food. The foraging drive in rats is so strong  that the animals readily work for food even when food is freely available. Commonly used ad libitum feeding  is associated with a reduced life span, increased incidence of tumours and risk of liver and kidney diseases.  It is also considered to be the most poorly controlled variable in rodent bioassays. The aim of this  study was to assess whether rats will gnaw wood in order to obtain food hidden in wooden walls, whether  this activity has a beneficial effect on controlling weight gain, and whether a typical diurnal activity rhythm  is maintained. A total of 18 BN/RijHsd and 18 F344/NHsd male rats were housed in either open or individually  ventilated cages (IVC), three rats in each cage. 10 of 36 were fitted with a telemetric transponder.  Four groups were used: two groups (diet board and plain board) with a maze made of two crossed aspen  boards, the third having a rectangular aspen tube. One maze was of plainboard, but the other included  drilled holes snugly loaded with food pellets, the “diet board”, such that the rats had to gnaw wood to reach  the food. The other two groups – and the controls – were fed ad libitum. The study used a crossover design  and the added item was changed every two weeks. Rats, added items, and amount of food left at the end of  the two week period were weighed. The statistical assessment showed that in terms of weight gain there  was a significant interaction both in IVC- (p = 0.005) and in open cages (p < 0.001) between the strains  and the group. In the F344 rats the diet board was more effective in controlling weight, but when combining  the strains, all comparisons with diet board were significant (p < 0.05). Use of strain and added item  as main effects, and age as covariate, showed that in the IVC-system there was a significant (p < 0.001)  interaction between the strain and the group, this effect being rather clear in the F344 rats in terms of  amount of food disappearing. In the open cage system, both strain and group were significant (p < 0.001)  factors; all three comparisons with diet board were significant (p < 0.001) in the amount of food disappearing.  In conclusion, the work-for-food approach appears to be a promising way of avoiding obesity  without causing untoward effects on diurnal activity in rats. Hence, the approach may have considerable  refinement and reduction potential.

    Exposure in the Shoebox: Comparison of Physical Environment of IVCs and Open Rat Cages

    Get PDF
    New caging and innovative items for more structured environment within the cage have been introduced.  Many of these innovations cannot be seen as 'pure' or individual procedures, but rather they represent a  mixed exposure with a multitude of operant factors, some possibly having an impact on animals and  research. One kind of new caging system is the individually ventilated cage (IVC), where each cage  receives its own non-contaminated airflow, primarily designed for health status maintenance and occupational  safety. Even though those cages may be the same as those used in open cage systems, the physical  environment inside the cage may not identical. Comparison between cage types is difficult without characterization  of the physical environment, because the change may involve alterations in several parameters  in the environment. The aim of this study is to characterize and compare common physical parameters in  the ordinary situation, where IVC-racks are kept in the same room with open cages. The cage type used  was a polysulfone solid bottom cage. The parameters measured in this study were: illumination, temperature,  relative humidity (RH) and acoustic level in both IVCs and open top cages. No animals were in the  cages during light intensity, but there was bedding in the cage during acoustic measurements and both bedding  as well as a half-full food hopper during the illumination measurements. The temperature and (RH)  measurements were carried out with three male rats in each cage. There were differences between IVCs  and open top cages in all measured parameters. The light intensity was lower in IVCs, most likely due to  more compact cage placement in the rack and the additional plastic cover lid of the cage. Both maximum  and minimum temperatures were 1-4 oC higher in IVCs; which suggests that their ventilation is incapable  of taking away heat, produced inside the cage. Similarly, the relative humidity was higher in the IVCs. The  sound level adjusted to rat's hearing with R-weighting was higher in IVCs when compared to open cages.  Furthermore, the sound level was highest in the corners next to the ventilation valves. In conclusion, there  may be differences between open cages with IVCs involving several physical parameters of cage environment  and this may confound comparisons between results obtained in these cage systems.

    Environmental enrichment may alter the number of rats needed to achieve statistical significance

    Get PDF
    Currently, environmental enrichment studies have focused on detection of differences in group means resulting from varied enrichments. The effects of enrichment procedures on variation and hence on study results have received far less attention. Within group variation in studies is directly related to adequate sample size and thus determines ethically correct number of animals to be used. The aim of this study was to evaluate the effects of enrichment items on clinical chemistryand physiological parameters of Wistar rats with the emphasis on the extent of variation. Fortyeight weaned female and male outbred Wistar rats from eight different litters were housed for five weeks in groups of four rats per cage with anaspen tube (20x12x12 em), an aspen block (6x6x6 cm) or without enrichment items (control animals). At the end of the study, adrenals and brown adipose tissues were weighed, growth calculated from initial and final body weights and thefollowing assays were performed from serum samples: AFOS, ALAT, ASAT, LDH, GGT, Pi, Ca, cholesterol, triglycerides, ereatinine, total bilirubin, protein and corticosterone. Based on the variation detected, SOLO Power Analysis was used to calculate the smallest number of animals required (n) to detect an arbitrarily chosen 20 % difference in all means, when significance was set at p=0.05 and statistical power at 0.90. Based on the n-values, additional N-ratio (nenrichment / ncontrol) was calculated to indicate, how many times more or less animals were needed in the enrichment group in comparison to the control group. The enrichment items did not have effects on group means. However. they seemed to influence the variation and hence on the number of animals needed, with enzyme parameters being the most sensitive in this respect. This relationship between enrichment, variation and number of animals needed should be kept in mind while designing experiments with ethically correct sample size

    Cage position preferences of rats

    Get PDF
    Polycarbonate and stainless steel are commonly used cage body materials for laboratory rodents. The aim of this study was to assess preference of rats for the cage material. Altogether 64 male rats were used, 32 in two different facilities. The study cages were made of either stainless steel with a polycarbonate false inner half (Kuopio) or polycarbonate with a steel false inner half (Oulu). There were four different options for alignment of false cage halves and food hopper, and likewisetwo options for which end of the cage faced the wall. A video camera with time lapse recording of one second each minute was used. Weekly recording started at 16.00 and ended at 01.30, and each cage was recorded when the rats were agedfour, five, six. seven and eight weeks, The results were processed separately for both facilities and for day and night. Statistical analysis was done with ANOVA using alignment of food hopper to false inner cage half and direction to the wall asthe main effects during the daytime the location of the rats in nontransparent steel body cages is largely governed by the light intensity created by cage walls and hopper, but stainless steel was clearly preferred in three of four possiblecombinations. During the night the element of tight direction became less important, but again hopper and steel combination was more attractive than the hopper and polycarbonate combination. In polyearbonate cages with false steel inner half cages: the light intensity difference had a less prominent role. However, during the day, placing the feed hopper with steel was preferred. During the night, the even distribution was indicative of a slight preference to the familiar wall material,polycarbonate. In conclusion, in both study sites stainless steel was favored in 3 out 4 possible combinations during the day, During the dark, when transparency of the material was less critical, animals accustomed to a stainless steel cagespreferred steel over polycarbonate, but for animals raised in polycarbonate cages neither steel nor polycarbonate was favored
    corecore