13 research outputs found

    Oscillating magnetic field disrupts magnetic orientation in Zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata

    Get PDF
    Background Zebra finches can be trained to use the geomagnetic field as a directional cue for short distance orientation. The physical mechanisms underlying the primary processes of magnetoreception are, however, largely unknown. Two hypotheses of how birds perceive magnetic information are mainly discussed, one dealing with modulation of radical pair processes in retinal structures, the other assuming that iron deposits in the upper beak of the birds are involved. Oscillating magnetic fields in the MHz range disturb radical pair mechanisms but do not affect magnetic particles. Thus, application of such oscillating fields in behavioral experiments can be used as a diagnostic tool to decide between the two alternatives. Methods In a setup that eliminates all directional cues except the geomagnetic field zebra finches were trained to search for food in the magnetic north/south axis. The birds were then tested for orientation performance in two magnetic conditions. In condition 1 the horizontal component of the geomagnetic field was shifted by 90 degrees using a helmholtz coil. In condition 2 a high frequently oscillating field (1.156 MHz) was applied in addition to the shifted field. Another group of birds was trained to solve the orientation task, but with visual landmarks as directional cue. The birds were then tested for their orientation performance in the same magnetic conditions as applied for the first experiment. Results The zebra finches could be trained successfully to orient in the geomagnetic field for food search in the north/south axis. They were also well oriented in test condition 1, with the magnetic field shifted horizontally by 90 degrees. In contrast, when the oscillating field was added the directional choices during food search were randomly distributed. Birds that were trained to visually guided orientation showed no difference of orientation performance in the two magnetic conditions

    Spatial Orientation in Japanese Quails (Coturnix coturnix japonica)

    Get PDF
    Finding a given location can be based on a variety of strategies, for example on the estimation of spatial relations between landmarks, called spatial orientation. In galliform birds, spatial orientation has been demonstrated convincingly in very young domestic chicks. We wanted to know whether adult Japanese quails (Coturnix coturnix japonica) without food deprivation are also able to use spatial orientation. The quails had to learn the relation of a food location with four conspicuous landmarks which were placed in the corners of a square shaped arena. They were trained to find mealworms in three adjacent food cups in a circle of 20 such cups. The rewarded feeders were located during training between the same two landmarks each of which showed a distinct pattern. When the birds had learned the task, all landmarks were displaced clockwise by 90 degrees. When tested in the new situation, all birds redirected their choices with respect to the landmark shift. In subsequent tests, however, the previously correct position was also chosen. According to our results, quails are using conspicuous landmarks as a first choice for orientation. The orientation towards the previously rewarded location, however, indicates that the neuronal representation of space which is used by the birds also includes more fine grain, less conspicuous cues, which are probably also taken into account in uncertain situations. We also presume that the rare orientation towards never rewarded feeders may be due to a foraging strategy instead of being mistakes

    Social background matters : the importance of adolescent peer experience for adult social behavior

    No full text
    Ruploh T. Social background matters : the importance of adolescent peer experience for adult social behavior. Bielefeld; 2014

    Captive domesticated zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) have increased plasma corticosterone concentrations in the absence of bathing water

    No full text
    Krause ET, Ruploh T. Captive domesticated zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) have increased plasma corticosterone concentrations in the absence of bathing water. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 2016;182:80-85.Keeping animals in captivity should always favour conditions that aim to improve their welfare withrespect to species-specific requirements. For laboratory animals, the majority of welfare issues havebeen explored in rodents thus far, whereas the effect of housing conditions on the well-being of avian labspecies has received relatively little attention. Here, we investigate the importance of access to a waterbath in captivity on the welfare of a drought-adapted passerine, the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata).Zebra finches can survive long periods of drought in the wild, which also includes a lack of surface waterfor bathing, but if water is available, they regularly take the opportunity to bathe in water. Water bathsrepresent an important comfort behaviour for zebra finches, especially because the birds do not take dustbaths. Here, we wanted to examine the role of water baths in relation to corticosterone concentrationsas an indicator of well-being in captive zebra finches. We sought to determine how important it is toprovide water baths to zebra finches in captivity. Therefore, we repeatedly quantified the basal plasmastress hormone levels, i.e., corticosterone (CORT), and the body weight of individuals over a three-monthperiod. During this time, control birds had permanent access to a water bath, while treatment birdsexperienced a 30-day period without the opportunity to bathe during the second month. We demonstratethat zebra finches lacking bathing opportunities show higher basal plasma CORT concentrations (GLM,p = 0.034) but do not differ in body weight in comparison to control birds (GLM, p = 0.31). Our resultsshow that even for birds that can tolerate long periods of drought in their natural habitat, access to awater bath is essential for their well-being and their welfare, and thus, water baths should be providedunder captive housing conditions. As chronically elevated stress hormone levels can have short-andlong-term detrimental effects, our findings have important implications for welfare considerations inthe management of one of the most used laboratory birds

    Effects of social conditions during adolescence on courtship and aggressive behavior are not abolished by adult social experience

    No full text
    Ruploh T, Bischof H-J, von Engelhardt N. Effects of social conditions during adolescence on courtship and aggressive behavior are not abolished by adult social experience. Bielefeld University; 2015

    Adolescent social environment shapes sexual and aggressive behaviour of adult male zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata).

    No full text
    Ruploh T, Bischof H-J, von Engelhardt N. Adolescent social environment shapes sexual and aggressive behaviour of adult male zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Bielefeld University; 2013

    Social Place Avoidance Learning in Zebra Finches

    No full text
    Ruploh T, Schiffhauer B, Bischof H-J. Social Place Avoidance Learning in Zebra Finches. In: Watanabe S, ed. Logic and Sensibility. Tokyo: Center for Advanced Research on Logic and Sensibility, The Global COE Program, Keio University; 2012: 39-50

    Effects of social conditions during adolescence on courtship and aggressive behavior are not abolished by adult social experience

    No full text
    Ruploh T, Henning M, Bischof H-J, von Engelhardt N. Effects of social conditions during adolescence on courtship and aggressive behavior are not abolished by adult social experience. Developmental Psychobiology. 2015;57(1):73-82

    Choice scores.

    No full text
    <p>Choice scores calculated from all ten test trials (N = 7, means ± SD). In the test phase, quails could choose between three alternatives. First, one of the actually accessible cups, second, a cup that was baited in the training sessions, third, a cup that was never baited before. For calculation of choice scores and statistics see text.</p

    Results.

    No full text
    <p>Black dots represent baited positions. A) Small arrows represent individual means calculated from the last ten training trials B) Small arrows depict individual choice directions in the first test trial. C) Small arrows show individual means of choice directions calculated from the ten test trials. D) Small arrows point to individual single choices in all ten test trials. A–C) The centrally based arrow depicts the mean vector calculated from the individual values.</p
    corecore