51 research outputs found

    Curvature Contrast in Stereoscopically-Defined Surfaces

    Get PDF

    The influence of spatial pattern on visual short-term memory for contrast

    Get PDF
    Several psychophysical studies of visual short-term memory (VSTM) have shown high-fidelity storage capacity for many properties of visual stimuli. On judgments of the spatial frequency of gratings, for example, discrimination performance does not decrease significantly, even for memory intervals of up to 30 s. For other properties, such as stimulus orientation and contrast, however, such “perfect storage” behavior is not found, although the reasons for this difference remain unresolved. Here, we report two experiments in which we investigated the nature of the representation of stimulus contrast in VSTM using spatially complex, two-dimensional random-noise stimuli. We addressed whether information about contrast per se is retained during the memory interval by using a test stimulus with the same spatial structure but either the same or the opposite local contrast polarity, with respect to the comparison (i.e., remembered) stimulus. We found that discrimination thresholds got steadily worse with increasing duration of the memory interval. Furthermore, performance was better when the test and comparison stimuli had the same local contrast polarity than when they were contrast-reversed. Finally, when a noise mask was introduced during the memory interval, its disruptive effect was maximal when the spatial configuration of its constituent elements was uncorrelated with those of the comparison and test stimuli. These results suggest that VSTMfor contrast is closely tied to the spatial configuration of stimuli and is not transformed into a more abstract representation

    GreekLex 2: a comprehensive lexical database with part-of-speech, syllabic, phonological, and stress information

    Get PDF
    Databases containing lexical properties on any given orthography are crucial for psycholinguistic research. In the last ten years, a number of lexical databases have been developed for Greek. However, these lack important part-of-speech information. Furthermore, the need for alternative procedures for calculating syllabic measurements and stress information, as well as combination of several metrics to investigate linguistic properties of the Greek language are highlighted. To address these issues, we present a new extensive lexical database of Modern Greek (GreekLex 2) with part-of-speech information for each word and accurate syllabification and orthographic information predictive of stress, as well as several measurements of word similarity and phonetic information. The addition of detailed statistical information about Greek part-of-speech, syllabification, and stress neighbourhood allowed novel analyses of stress distribution within different grammatical categories and syllabic lengths to be carried out. Results showed that the statistical preponderance of stress position on the pre-final syllable that is reported for Greek language is dependent upon grammatical category. Additionally, analyses showed that a proportion higher than 90% of the tokens in the database would be stressed correctly solely by relying on stress neighbourhood information. The database and the scripts for orthographic and phonological syllabification as well as phonetic transcription are available at http://www.psychology.nottingham.ac.uk/greeklex/

    On the Inverse Problem of Binocular 3D Motion Perception

    Get PDF
    It is shown that existing processing schemes of 3D motion perception such as interocular velocity difference, changing disparity over time, as well as joint encoding of motion and disparity, do not offer a general solution to the inverse optics problem of local binocular 3D motion. Instead we suggest that local velocity constraints in combination with binocular disparity and other depth cues provide a more flexible framework for the solution of the inverse problem. In the context of the aperture problem we derive predictions from two plausible default strategies: (1) the vector normal prefers slow motion in 3D whereas (2) the cyclopean average is based on slow motion in 2D. Predicting perceived motion directions for ambiguous line motion provides an opportunity to distinguish between these strategies of 3D motion processing. Our theoretical results suggest that velocity constraints and disparity from feature tracking are needed to solve the inverse problem of 3D motion perception. It seems plausible that motion and disparity input is processed in parallel and integrated late in the visual processing hierarchy

    The detection of second-order motion in the human visual system

    No full text
    Available from British Library Document Supply Centre-DSC:DX192540 / BLDSC - British Library Document Supply CentreSIGLEGBUnited Kingdo

    Changes in perceived speed following adaptation to first-order and second-order motion

    No full text
    To investigate whether or not adaptation to second-order motion can cause changes in perceived speed, measurements of perceived speed were obtained for two varieties of motion: (i) contrast-modulated two-dimensional static noise (second-order motion); and (ii) luminance-modulated noise (first-order motion). The test stimulus (either first-order or second-order) was presented to one side of a central fixation spot and a comparison stimulus (always first-order) was simultaneously presented on the opposite side. The observer's task was to indicate which of the two motion stimuli appeared to drift faster. The perceived speed of the test stimulus was measured with and without prior adaptation to motion on one side of the fixation spot only (that of the test stimulus). The modulation depth of the adaptation stimulus was always half that of the test stimulus and all test patterns were equated for visibility. The pattern of results for second-order motion was similar to that for first-order motion. Typically, adaptation reduced perceived speed, particularly when the adaptation speed was faster than the test speed. However, when the adaptation speed was low relative to the test speed, increases in perceived speed were found. Cross-over adaptation effects between first-order and second-order motion were also observed. Robust velocity aftereffects were found for second-order motion when the noise was dynamic or was high-pass filtered, suggesting that first-order (luminance) artifacts were not responsible for the velocity aftereffects observed. We conclude that the perceived speeds of first-order and second-order motion appear to be encoded in human vision using similar computational principles (but not necessarily utilizing the same mechanism), since the same pattern of results was found for the two varieties of motion

    Changes in perceived speed following adaptation to first-order and second-order motion

    No full text
    To investigate whether or not adaptation to second-order motion can cause changes in perceived speed, measurements of perceived speed were obtained for two varieties of motion: (i) contrast-modulated two-dimensional static noise (second-order motion); and (ii) luminance-modulated noise (first-order motion). The test stimulus (either first-order or second-order) was presented to one side of a central fixation spot and a comparison stimulus (always first-order) was simultaneously presented on the opposite side. The observer's task was to indicate which of the two motion stimuli appeared to drift faster. The perceived speed of the test stimulus was measured with and without prior adaptation to motion on one side of the fixation spot only (that of the test stimulus). The modulation depth of the adaptation stimulus was always half that of the test stimulus and all test patterns were equated for visibility. The pattern of results for second-order motion was similar to that for first-order motion. Typically, adaptation reduced perceived speed, particularly when the adaptation speed was faster than the test speed. However, when the adaptation speed was low relative to the test speed, increases in perceived speed were found. Cross-over adaptation effects between first-order and second-order motion were also observed. Robust velocity aftereffects were found for second-order motion when the noise was dynamic or was high-pass filtered, suggesting that first-order (luminance) artifacts were not responsible for the velocity aftereffects observed. We conclude that the perceived speeds of first-order and second-order motion appear to be encoded in human vision using similar computational principles (but not necessarily utilizing the same mechanism), since the same pattern of results was found for the two varieties of motion.Copyright 1997 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. Re-use of this article is permitted in accordance with the Terms and Conditions set out at http://www.elsevier.com/open-access/userlicense/1.0
    corecore