25 research outputs found

    People are more likely to describe a violent event as terrorism if the perpetrator is Muslim and has policy goals

    Get PDF
    Recent mass shootings in the US have provoked debate over what should be considered to be "terrorism". In new research Connor Huff and Joshua D. Kertzer explore these public debates via a survey which seeks to determine which kinds of incidents - and perpetrators - mean that people are more likely to classify an event as terrorism. They find that ..

    Taking Foreign Policy Personally: Personal Values and Foreign Policy Attitudes

    Get PDF
    Previous research has shown that on issues of foreign policy, individuals have “general stances,” “postures,” “dispositions” or “orientations” that inform their beliefs toward more discrete issues in international relations. While these approaches delineate the proximate sources of public opinion in the foreign policy domain, they evade an even more important question: what gives rise to these foreign policy orientations in the first place? Combining an original survey on a nationally representative sample of Americans with Schwartz’s theory of values from political psychology, we show that people take foreign policy personally: the same basic values we know people use to guide choices in their daily lives also travel to the domain of foreign affairs, offering one potential explanation why people who are otherwise uninformed about world politics nonetheless express coherent foreign policy beliefs

    Response to Roseanne W. McManus’s review of Resolve in International Politics

    No full text

    Making Sense of Isolationism: Foreign Policy Mood as a Multilevel Phenomenon

    No full text

    Replication Data for: Decomposing Audience Costs: Bringing the Audience Back into Audience Cost Theory

    No full text
    According to a growing tradition in International Relations, one way governments can credibly signal their intentions in foreign policy crises is by creating domestic audience costs: leaders can tie their hands by publicly threatening to use force, since domestic publics punish leaders who say one thing and do another. We argue here that there are actually two logics of audience costs: audiences can punish leaders both for being inconsistent (the traditional audience cost), and for threatening to use force in the first place (a belligerence cost). We employ an experiment that disentangles these two rationales, and turn to a series of dispositional characteristics from political psychology to bring the audience back into audience cost theory. Our results suggest that traditional audience cost experiments may overestimate how much people care about inconsistency, and that the logic of audience costs (and the implications for crisis bargaining) varies considerably with the leader's constituency

    Supplemental Material - Armies and Influence: Elite Experience and Public Opinion on Foreign Policy

    No full text
    Supplemental Material Armies and Influence: Elite Experience and Public Opinion on Foreign Policy by Tyler Jost and Joshua D. Kertzer in Journal of Conflict Resolution</p

    Supplemental Material - Armies and Influence: Elite Experience and Public Opinion on Foreign Policy

    No full text
    Supplemental Material Armies and Influence: Elite Experience and Public Opinion on Foreign Policy by Tyler Jost and Joshua D. Kertzer in Journal of Conflict Resolution</p

    Folk Realism: Testing the Microfoundations of Realism in Ordinary Citizens

    No full text
    International Relations scholars have long debated whether the American public is allergic to realism, which raises the question of how they would “contract” it in the first place. We argue that realism isn't just an IR paradigm, but a belief system, whose relationship with other ideological systems in public opinion has rarely been fully examined. Operationalizing this disposition in ordinary citizens as “folk realism,” we investigate its relationship with a variety of personality traits, foreign policy orientations, and political knowledge. We then present the results of a laboratory experiment probing psychological microfoundations for realist theory, manipulating the amount of information subjects have about a foreign policy conflict to determine whether uncertainty leads individuals to adopt more realist views, and whether realists and idealists respond to uncertainty and fear differently. We find that many of realism's causal mechanisms are conditional on whether subjects already hold realist views, and suggest that emotions like fear may play a larger role in realist theory than many realists have assumed
    corecore