990 research outputs found

    The drive to control : how affect and motivation regulate cognitive control

    Get PDF
    The studies described in this thesis aimed to investigate how affect and motivation impact cognitive control, in terms of both behavior and brain activation. Six out of the eight empirical studies found support for indirect effects on cognitive control, as measured with sequential trial-to-trial adaptations in cognitive control tasks. Only two studies resulted in evidence for a direct modulation of cognitive control (Chapter 4 and 9). Indirect effects occurred on trial-to-trial adaptation in cognitive control tasks involving a random presentation of compatible and incompatible trials. We found that conflict adaptation, the transient improvement of behavioral control after incompatible in comparison to compatible trials, was subject to affective regulation. In particular, we found that after incompatible trials, positive emotional states reduced and negative emotional states increased adaptation. These effects occurred for both short-term (Chapters 2 and 3) and long-term affect manipulations (Chapters 5, 6, and 7). Motivation and task difficulty also interacted with conflict adaptation (Chapter 8). The neuroimaging studies described in Chapter 3 and 6 demonstrate the role of fronto-striatal interactions in this affective regulation of cognitive control. Taken together, this thesis demonstrates the role that positive and negative emotions play in the adaptation of behavior and mental effort.LEI Universiteit LeidenFSW - Action Control - OudAction Contro

    Threat But Not Arousal Narrows Attention: Evidence from Pupil Dilation and Saccade Control

    Get PDF
    It has been shown that negative affect causes attentional narrowing. According to Easterbrook’s (1959) influential hypothesis this effect is driven by the withdrawal motivation inherent to negative emotions and might be related to increases in arousal. We investigated whether valence-unspecific increases in physiological arousal, as measured by pupil dilation, could account for attentional narrowing effects in a cognitive control task. Following the presentation of a negative, positive, or neutral picture, participants performed a saccade task with a pro-saccade versus an anti-saccade instruction. The reaction time difference between pro- and anti-saccades was used to index attentional selectivity, and while pupil diameter was used as an index of physiological arousal. Pupil dilation was observed for both negative and positive pictures, which indicates increased physiological arousal. However, increased attentional selectivity was only observed following negative pictures. Our data show that motivational intensity effects on attentional narrowing can occur independently of physiological arousal effects

    QRTEngine: An easy solution for running online reaction time experiments using Qualtrics

    Get PDF
    Performing online behavioral research is gaining increased popularity among researchers in psychological and cognitive science. However, the currently available methods for conducting online reaction time experiments are often complicated and typically require advanced technical skills. In this article, we introduce the Qualtrics Reaction Time Engine (QRTEngine), an open-source JavaScript engine that can be embedded in the online survey development environment Qualtrics. The QRTEngine can be used to easily develop browser-based online reaction time experiments with accurate timing within current browser capabilities, and it requires only minimal programming skills. After introducing the QRTEngine, we briefly discuss how to create and distribute a Stroop task. Next, we describe a study in which we investigated the timing accuracy of the engine under different processor loads using external chronometry. Finally, we show that the QRTEngine can be used to reproduce classic behavioral effects in three reaction time paradigms: a Stroop task, an attentional blink task, and a masked-priming task. These findings demonstrate that QRTEngine can be used as a tool for conducting online behavioral research even when this requires accurate stimulus presentation times

    Passive element enriched photoacoustic computed tomography (PER PACT) for simultaneous imaging of acoustic propagation properties and light absorption\ud

    Get PDF
    We present a ‘hybrid’ imaging approach which can image both light absorption properties and acoustic transmission properties of an object in a two-dimensional slice using a computed tomography (CT) photoacoustic imager. The ultrasound transmission measurement method uses a strong optical absorber of small cross-section placed in the path of the light illuminating the sample. This absorber, which we call a passive element acts as a source of ultrasound. The interaction of ultrasound with the sample can be measured in transmission, using the same ultrasound detector used for photoacoustics. Such measurements are made at various angles around the sample in a CT approach. Images of the ultrasound propagation parameters, attenuation and speed of sound, can be reconstructed by inversion of a measurement model. We validate the method on specially designed phantoms and biological specimens. The obtained images are quantitative in terms of the shape, size, location, and acoustic properties of the examined heterogeneitie

    Promises and pitfalls of Web-based experimentation in the advance of replicable psychological science. A reply to Plant (2015)

    Get PDF
    In a recent letter, Plant (2015) reminded us that proper calibration of our laboratory experiments is important for the progress of psychological science. Therefore, carefully controlled laboratory studies are argued to be preferred over Web-based experimentation, in which timing is usually more imprecise. Here we argue that there are many situations in which the timing of Web-based experimentation is acceptable and that online experimentation provides a very useful and promising complementary toolbox to available lab-based approaches. We discuss examples in which stimulus calibration or calibration against response criteria is necessary and situations in which this is not critical. We also discuss how online labor markets, such as Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, allow researchers to acquire data in more diverse populations and to test theories along more psychological dimensions. Recent methodological advances that have produced more accurate browser-based stimulus presentation are also discussed. In our view, online experimentation is one of the most promising avenues to advance replicable psychological science in the near future.Action Contro

    Pupil dilation in the Simon task as a marker of conflict processing

    Get PDF
    Cognitive demands in response conflict paradigms trigger negative affect and avoidance behavior. However, not all response conflict studies show increases in physiological indices of emotional arousal, such as pupil diameter. In contrast to earlier null-results, this study shows for the first time that small (about 0.02 mm) conflict-related pupil dilation can be observed in a Simon task when stimuli do not introduce a light reflex. Results show that response-conflict in Simon trials induces both pupil dilation and reaction-time costs. Moreover, sequential analyses reveal that pupil dilation mirrors the conflict-adaptation pattern observed in reaction time (RT). Although single-trial regression analyses indicated that pupil dilation is likely to reflect more than one process at the same time, in general our findings imply that pupil dilation can be used as an indirect marker of conflict processing.215Action Contro

    Patients with obesity have more inflamed joints and higher CRP levels during the disease course in ACPA-positive RA but not in ACPA-negative RA

    Get PDF
    BACKGROUND: Obese RA patients have higher disease activity scores (DAS). Previous research showed that obese RA patients have higher tender joint count (TJC) and VAS general health. However, it remains unclear whether DAS components measuring local and systemic inflammation (swollen joint count (SJC), CRP) are increased and if this is present in the total RA population or confined to an ACPA subgroup. As ACPA is suggested to enhance inflammatory responses, we hypothesized that the association of obesity with SJC and CRP is present especially in ACPA-positive RA. We therefore studied associations of obesity with courses of DAS components in ACPA subgroups. METHODS: We studied 649 RA patients (291 ACPA-positive), included in the Leiden Early Arthritis Clinic. Five-year courses of DAS44 and DAS44 components (SJC-44, TJC-53, CRP, VAS (0-100)) were compared between RA patients with normal weight (BMI 18.5-24.9), overweight (25.0-29.9), and obesity (≥ 30.0), stratified for ACPA. Linear/Poisson mixed models with a knot at 4 months were used. RESULTS: Obese RA patients had + 0.32 higher DAS compared to normal weight during the 5-year follow-up. In ACPA-positive RA, obese patients had + 0.43 (95% CI: 0.22, 0.64) higher DAS, whereas in ACPA-negative RA, this difference was smaller and not statistically significant: + 0.19 (95% CI: - 0.01, 0.38). In ACPA-positive RA, all DAS components were significantly higher in obese patients compared to normal weight: SJC + 60% (IRR1.60; 95% CI: 1.18, 2.16), CRP + 3.7 mg/L (95% CI:0.95, 6.53), TJC + 55% (IRR1.55; 95% CI:1.15, 2.10), and VAS + 9 (95% CI: 4.0, 14.2). ACPA-negative obese RA patients tended to have higher TJC (IRR1.22; 95% CI: 0.96, 1.55) and VAS (β4.3; 95% CI: - 0.4, 9.0), while SJC (IRR1.07; 95% CI:0.85, 1.33) and CRP (β0.24; 95% CI: - 1.29, 3.32) were unaffected. CONCLUSION: The association of obesity with a worse DAS course is mainly present in ACPA-positive RA; especially SJC and CRP levels remain higher in ACPA-positive RA patients with obesity but not ACPA-negative RA patients. This is the first demonstration that obesity influences the disease course of ACPA-positive and ACPA-negative RA differently.</p
    corecore