57 research outputs found
Parental use of âcry it outâ in infants: no adverse effects on attachment and behavioural development at 18 months
Background
Leaving infant to cry it out has been the subject of discussion among researchers and parents. Nevertheless, there is paucity of empirical research investigating the association between leaving infant to cry it out and consequent crying duration and frequency, motherâinfant attachment and behavioural development.
Methods
The sample with complete longitudinal data comprised 178 infants and their caretakers. Parental use of âleaving infant to cry outâ and cry duration were assessed with maternal report at term, 3, 6 and 18 months, and frequency of crying was assessed at term, 3 and 18 months of age. Attachment was measured at 18 months using the strange situation procedure. Behavioural development of the infant was assessed with two observational measures and a parentâreport questionnaire at 18 months.
Results
The use of âleaving infant to cryingâ was rare at term and increased over the next 18 months. âLeaving infants to cry it outâ at term was associated with a decrease in crying frequency at 3 months. Furthermore, leaving infants to cry it out a few times at term and often at 3 months was associated with shorter fuss/cry duration at 18 months of age. No adverse impacts of leaving infants to cry it out in the first 6 months on infantâmother attachment and behavioural development at 18 months were found.
Conclusions
Contemporary practice of some parents in the United Kingdom to occasionally or often âleaving infant to cry it outâ during the first 6 months was not associated with adverse behavioural development and attachment at 18 months. Increased use of âleaving to cry it outâ with age may indicate differential responding by parents related to infant selfâregulation
Justice sensitivity and rejection sensitivity as predictors and outcomes of eating disorder pathology: A 5âyear longitudinal study
Objective:
Rejection sensitivity and justice sensitivity are personality traits that are characterized by frequent perceptions and intense adverse responses to negative social cues. Whereas there is good evidence for associations between rejection sensitivity, justice sensitivity, and internalizing problems, no longitudinal studies have investigated their association with eating disorder (ED) pathology so far. Thus, the present study examined longitudinal relations between rejection sensitivity, justice sensitivity, and ED pathology.
Method:
Participants (N = 769) reported on their rejection sensitivity, justice sensitivity, and ED pathology at 9-19 (T1), 11-21 (T2), and 14-22 years of age (T3).
Results:
Latent cross-lagged models showed longitudinal associations between ED pathology and anxious rejection sensitivity, observer and victim justice sensitivity. T1 and T2 ED pathology predicted higher T2 and T3 anxious rejection sensitivity, respectively. In turn, T2 anxious rejection sensitivity predicted more T3 ED pathology. T1 observer justice sensitivity predicted more T2 ED pathology, which predicted higher T3 observer justice sensitivity. Furthermore, T1 ED pathology predicted higher T2 victim justice sensitivity.
Discussion:
Rejection sensitivity-particularly anxious rejection sensitivity-and justice sensitivity may be involved in the maintenance or worsening of ED pathology and should be considered by future research and in prevention and treatment of ED pathology. Also, mental health problems may increase rejection sensitivity and justice sensitivity traits in the long term
Infant regulatory problems : neurodevelopmental vulnerability and sensitive parenting
Infant regulatory problems (crying, sleeping, feeding) are a common concern for parents and practitioners. Although there is now good evidence of the long-term adverse influences of infant regulatory problems on mental health, in particular if they co-occur together (multiple regulatory problems), important gaps remain regarding the precursors of regulatory problems. In particular, it is unclear whether and how sensitive parenting and/or neurodevelopmental vulnerability are involved in the development of multiple regulatory problems. Furthermore, do regulatory problems impair the development of the infantsâ relationship to their mothers, i.e. attachment? This thesis explores neurodevelopmental vulnerability and sensitive parenting as precursors of multiple regulatory problems, and whether multiple regulatory problems increase the likelihood of insecure and/or disorganised attachment.
The thesis consists of four studies and uses preterm birth as a natural model to assess neurodevelopmental vulnerability due to the interruption caused by preterm birth on the key processes of brain development. Study 1, a meta-analysis, explored the relationship between neurodevelopmental vulnerability and maternal sensitivity by comparing maternal sensitivity in preterm and full-term infants. Findings indicate that having an infant with neurodevelopmental vulnerability does not alter mothersâ sensitive parenting. In Study 2, using the Growth of at risk Infants (GAIN) study, the effect of neurodevelopmental vulnerability on regulatory problems across the first 18 months was investigated. Very preterm/very low birth weight infants experienced more multiple regulatory problems at term and 18 months compared to full-term infants. In Study 3, the longitudinal relationship between neurodevelopmental vulnerability, maternal sensitivity and multiple regulatory problems across infancy was explored allowing for reciprocal associations between maternal sensitivity and multiple regulatory problems across infancy. Both maternal sensitivity and multiple regulatory problems were moderately persistent from term to 18 months. Consistent with our previous findings, it was revealed that neurodevelopmental vulnerability had an enduring impact on multiple regulatory problems. On the other hand, maternal sensitivity at term had only a short-term negative impact on multiple regulatory problems at 3 months. No evidence for a reciprocal influence of maternal sensitivity and multiple regulatory problems was found. Finally, Study 4 examined whether early multiple regulatory problems at 3 and 6 months increase the likelihood of insecure and/or disorganised attachment. Findings revealed that multiple regulatory problems as early as 3 months increased the risk of both insecure and in particular, disorganised attachment at 18 months.
In conclusion, neurodevelopmental vulnerability increases the risk of multiple regulatory problems, which are moderately persistent across the first 18 months of life. Furthermore, multiple regulatory problems do not impair maternal sensitivity but have adverse effects on the infantsâ relationship with their mothers by increasing the risk of insecure and disorganised attachment. Clinicians should be aware that multiple regulatory problems are a significant potential risk factor for poorer infant-mother relationship
Maternal Depression and Sleep Problems in Early Childhood: A Meta-Analysis.
Both prenatal and postnatal maternal depression have been associated with increased sleep problems in early childhood. However, this association is less consistent for postnatal depression, and the strength of the association remains unclear. The aim of the current study was to provide a quantitative synthesis of the literature to estimate the magnitude of the association between maternal depression and sleep problems in early childhood. Medline, PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, Web of Science, and Scopus were searched for prospective longitudinal studies from 1970 to December 2022. Of 117 articles screened, 22 studies met the inclusion criteria. Both prenatal depression (ORâ=â1.82; 95% CIâ=â1.28-2.61) and postnatal depression (ORâ=â1.65; 95% CIâ=â1.50-1.82) were associated with increased likelihood of sleep problems in early childhood. The heterogeneity between the studies was significant and high both for prenatal (Qâ=â432.323; I2â=â97.456, Pâ2â=â65.594, Pâ<â.001), which mean that conclusions are tentative and need to be considered within the possible influence of unmeasured confounding. However, mitigating depression symptoms in mothers both during pregnancy and in the postnatal period would be an effective strategy for reducing sleep problems in children
Commentary: Response to commentary by Davis and Kramer
Davis and Kramer (2021) in their commentary on our study (Bilgin & Wolke, 2020) state that we âargue that leaving an infant to âcry it outâ, rather than responding to the childâs cries, had no adverse effects on motherâinfant attachment at 18 monthsâ (Davis & Kramer, 2021, p. 1). Instead, we wrote that âcontemporary practice by some parents to occasionally or often âleaving infant to cry it outâ during the first 6 months was not associated with adverse behavioural development and attachment at 18 monthsâ (p. 8). Based on the empirical findings of our observation study, we suggested that âincreased use of âleaving to cry it outâ with age may indicate differential responding by mothers to aid the development of infant selfâregulationâ (p. 8). Indeed, in an editorial of our study, the joint editor of this journal concluded that âBilgin and Wolke responsibly conclude that there is little reason to make definitive pronouncements to parents of young infants about how much to let them cry it out, given that both the attachment theory (responding promptly early promotes security) and learning theory (ignoring crying prevents dependency) formulations were unsupported by their findingsâ (Zeanah, 2020, p. 1172)
Systematic review and meta-analysis : fussing and crying durations and prevalence of colic in infants
Objective
To determine the mean duration of fussing and crying and prevalence of colic using modified Wessel criteria in infants in the first 3 months of life.
Study design
A systematic literature search was performed using the databases Medline, PsycINFO, and Embase. The major outcome measure was mean total fuss/cry duration during 24 hours at ages 1-2 weeks (11 samples), 3-4 weeks (6 samples), 5-6 weeks (28 samples), 8-9 weeks (9 samples), and 10-12 weeks (12 samples).
Results
Of 5687 articles reviewed, 28 diary studies (33 samples) were suitable for inclusion in meta-analysis; these studies included 8690 infants. No statistical evidence for a universal crying peak at 6 weeks of age across studies was found. Rather, the mean fuss/cry duration across studies was stable at 117-133 minutes (SDs: 66-70) in the first 6 weeks and dropped to a mean of 68 minutes (SD: 46.2) by 10-12 weeks of age. Colic was much more frequent in the first 6 weeks (17%-25%) compared with 11% by 8-9 weeks of age and 0.6% by 10-12 weeks of age, according to modified Wessel criteria and lowest in Denmark and Japan.
Conclusions
The duration of fussing/crying drops significantly after 8-9 weeks of age, with colic as defined by modified Wessel criteria being rare in infants older than 9 weeks. Colic or excessive fuss/cry may be more accurately identified by defining fuss/cry above the 90th percentile in the chart provided based on the review
Longitudinal associations between justice sensitivity, nonsuicidal self-injury, substance use, and victimization by peers
Justice sensitivity (JS), the tendency to perceive and negatively respond to alleged injustice, has been associated with a range of internalizing and externalizing problems and peer victimization; however, it remains unclear if it has an association with self-victimization. Participants (N = 769) reported on their JS longitudinally at 9â19 (T1), 11â21 (T2), and 14â22 years of age (T3). They further reported on nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) and illegal substance use as indicators of self-victimization as well as victimization by peers at T2 and T3. A cross-lagged latent model revealed that victim JS at T1 was positively associated with NSSI, substance use, and peer victimization at T2, and victim JS at T2 was positively associated with substance use at T3. Higher observer JS at T2 predicted higher illegal substance use at T3 and higher illegal substance use at T2 predicted higher observer JS at T3. Finally, higher peer victimization at T2 predicted less perpetrator JS at T3 in the total group. Multigroup models further revealed sex-specific effects. Our findings highlight that being sensitive to injustice, particularly the tendency to feel unfairly treated or being taken advantage of, contributes to individualsâ vulnerability to both engaging in behaviors reflecting self-victimization and being a target of peer victimization, which in turn have influences on JS
Parentsâ life satisfaction prior to and following preterm birth
The current study tested whether the reported lower wellbeing of parents after preterm birth, relative to term birth, is a continuation of a pre-existing diference before pregnancy. Parents from Germany (the German Socio-Economic Panel Study, N= 10,649) and the United Kingdom (British Household Panel Study and Understanding Society, N= 11,012) reported their new-bornâs birthweight and gestational age, subsequently categorised as very preterm or very low birthweight (VP/VLBW, < 32 weeks or < 1500 g), moderately/late preterm or low birthweight (MLP/LBW, â„32 weeks and < 37 weeks/â„ 1500 g and < 2500 g), or term-born (â„ 37 weeks and â„ 2500 g). Mixed models were used to analyse life satisfaction, an aspect of wellbeing, at four assessments-two years and six months before birth and six months and two years afterwards. Two years before birth, satisfaction of prospective term-born, MLP/LBW, or VP/VLBW mothers did not signifcantly difer. However, mothers of VP/ VLBWs had lower satisfaction relative to mothers of term-borns at both assessments post-birth. Among fathers, satisfaction levels were similarly equivalent two years before birth. Subsequently, fathers of VP/VLBWs temporarily difered in satisfaction six months post-birth relative to fathers of term-borns. Results indicate that parentsâ lower life satisfaction after VP/VLBW birth is not a continuation of pre-existing life satisfaction diferences
Problems in peer relationships and low engagement in romantic relationships in preterm born adolescents: effects of maternal warmth in early childhood
This study examined whether maternal warmth in early childhood moderates the association between preterm birth and problems in peer relationships and low engagement in romantic relationships in adolescence. We studied 9193 individuals from the Millennium Cohort Study in the United Kingdom, 99 (1.1%) of whom were born very preterm (VPT; < 32Â weeks of gestation) and 629 (6.8%) moderate-to-late preterm (MLPT; 32â36Â weeks gestation). Maternal warmth was reported by the mothers when their children were 3Â years old. Peer relationship problems were reported by both the participants and their mothers at 14 and 17Â years. Further, participants reported their engagement in romantic relationships at 14 and 17Â years. All outcome variables were z-standardized, and the moderation effect was examined via hierarchical linear regressions. Compared to full-term birth, both MLPT and VPT birth were associated with lower engagement in romantic relationships at 17Â years of age (b =.04, p =.02; b =.11, p =.02, respectively), and VPT birth was associated with increased peer relationship problems at 14 (b =.29, p =.01) and 17Â years of age (b =.22, p =.046). Maternal warmth in early childhood was similarly associated with lower peer relationship problems in MLPT, VPT and full-term born adolescents. However, there was no influence of maternal warmth on engagement in romantic relationships at 17Â years of age. There is no major modifying effect of maternal warmth in early childhood on the association between PT birth and peer relationship problems and low engagement in romantic relationships at 14 and 17Â years of ages
The prevalence of personality disorders in the community: a global systematic review and meta-analysis
Background
Personality disorders are now internationally recognised as a mental health priority. Nevertheless, there are no systematic reviews examining the global prevalence of personality disorders.
Aims
To calculate the worldwide prevalence of personality disorders and examine whether rates vary between high and low and middle-income countries (LAMICs).
Method
We systematically searched PsycINFO, MEDLINE, EMBASE and PubMed from 1980 to May 2018 to identify articles reporting personality disorder prevalence rates in community populations (PROSPERO registration number: CRD42017065094).
Results
Forty-six studies (from 21 different countries spanning six continents) satisfied inclusion criteria. The worldwide pooled prevalence of any personality disorder was 7.8 % (95% Confidence Intervals: 6.1-9.5). Rates were greater in high income (9.6%; 95% CI: 7.9-11.3%) compared with LAMI (4.3%; 95% CI =2.6-6.1%) countries. In univariate meta-regressions, significant heterogeneity was partly attributable to study design (two-stage versus one-stage assessment), county income (high versus LAMI) and interview administration (clinician versus trained graduate). In multiple meta-regression analysis, study design remained a significant predictor of heterogeneity. Global rates of Cluster-A, B and C personality disorders were 3.8 % (3.2, 4.4%), 2.8% (1.6, 3.7%) and 5.0% (4.2, 5.9%).
Conclusions
Personality disorders are prevalent globally. Nevertheless, pooled prevalence rates should be interpreted with caution due to high levels of heterogeneity. More large-scale studies with standardised methodologies are now needed to increase our understanding of population needs and regional variations
- âŠ