106 research outputs found

    Role of colonoscopy in the diagnosis and treatment of pediatric lower gastrointestinal disorders

    Get PDF
    The safety and effectiveness of colonoscopy in the investigation of lower gastrointestinal tract pathology in children has been established for more than 2 decades in Korea. Skill and experience have since advanced to the point that both diagnostic and therapeutic colonoscopy are now routinely performed by most pediatric gastroenterologists. Pediatric colonoscopy differs significantly from its adult parallels in nearly every aspect including patient and parent management and preparation, selection criteria for sedation and general anesthetic, bowel preparation, expected diagnoses, instrument selection, imperative for terminal ileal intubation, and requirement for biopsies from macroscopically normal mucosa. Investigation of inflammatory bowel disease, whether for diagnosis or follow-up evaluation, and suspected colonic polyps are the most common indication for pediatric colonoscopy. The child who presents with signs and symptoms of lower gastrointestinal disorder should undergo colonoscopy with biopsy to make the diagnosis, as well as to help determine the appropriate therapy. This review introduces practical information on pediatric colonoscopy, the author's experiences, and the role of colonoscopic examination in the diagnosis and treatment of pediatric lower gastrointestinal disorders

    Programa de formaciĂłn complementaria para doctorandos: AplicaciĂłn de las nuevas tecnologĂ­as y redes sociales (FOCODO)

    Get PDF
    Este proyecto de innovaciĂłn docente va dirigido especĂ­ficamente a los alumnos de doctorado de las Facultades de Óptica y optometrĂ­a y de Veterinaria de la Universidad Complutense. Pero tambiĂ©n es Desarrollar un programa de formaciĂłn complementaria para los doctorandos en ciencias de la salud que les permita adquirir conocimiento y habilidades en ĂĄreas paralelas al ĂĄmbito en el que estĂĄn realizando sus tesis doctorales. El programa de formaciĂłn tendrĂĄ 4 areas temĂĄticas diferenciadas: - Habilidades de comunicaciĂłn (como escribir un artĂ­culo cientĂ­fico, como hacer presentaciones orales y en poster, como escribir un Proyecto cientĂ­fico, como trabajar en equipo, 
) - Conocimiento complementario a la tesis doctoral (estadĂ­stica, metodologĂ­a cientĂ­fica, lectura crĂ­tica de bibliografĂ­a, Buenas prĂĄcticas de laboratorio, buenas prĂĄcticas clĂ­nicas, 
) - Salidas profesionales durante y despuĂ©s de la tesis doctoral (Becas pre y postdoctorales, bĂșsqueda de proyectos de investigaciĂłn, como colaborar con otros grupos de investigaciĂłn,
) - FormaciĂłn especifica en su ĂĄrea temĂĄtica (tĂ©cnicas de laboratorio, tĂ©cnicas clĂ­nicas, revisiones bibliogrĂĄficas,
) Se generarĂĄ material de tres tipos: - Seminarios.- Son presentaciones de una hora de duraciĂłn sobre temas especĂ­ficos a los que se puede asistir en directo o poder ver grabado a travĂ©s de YouTube. Estos seminarios serĂĄn impartidos por profesores e investigadores de los grupos de investigacion que hacen la propuesta ademĂĄs de contar con especialistas nacionales e internacionales - Pildoras.- Videos cortos de no mas de 3 minutos que resuman los aspectos mas importantes de cada seminario que serĂĄn colgados en las pagina de los grupos de investigacion que se creen en las redes sociales. Estas pĂ­ldoras serĂĄn grabadas utilizando herramientas de grabaciĂłn habituales para videos en redes sociales (croma, focos, 
) - Summer/Winter School.- Curos de verano y/o invierno donde en dos-tres dĂ­as intensivos se imparta formacion especifica en investigacion. En estos cursos participaran profesores y expertos internacionales y estarĂĄ abierto para la matriculaciĂłn de estudiantes no complutenses, tanto nacionales como internacionales. Para llevar a cabo este programa se utilizarĂĄn varias herramientas: - Plataformas digitales (YouTube) donde se colgarĂĄn los seminarios que se realicen. - Redes sociales (Instagram, Twitter,
) donde se colgarĂĄn las pĂ­ldoras que se realicen y ademĂĄs se harĂĄn reuniones en directo para debatir temas de alguna de las ĂĄreas propuestas anteriormente. - App para mĂłvil desde donde los alumnos puedan encontrar todo el material que se genere durante el programa. Este app puede ser gratuita para los alumnos complutenses y de pago para el resto.Depto. de OptometrĂ­a y VisiĂłnFac. de Óptica y OptometrĂ­aFALSEsubmitte

    Clonal chromosomal mosaicism and loss of chromosome Y in elderly men increase vulnerability for SARS-CoV-2

    Full text link
    The pandemic caused by severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2, COVID-19) had an estimated overall case fatality ratio of 1.38% (pre-vaccination), being 53% higher in males and increasing exponentially with age. Among 9578 individuals diagnosed with COVID-19 in the SCOURGE study, we found 133 cases (1.42%) with detectable clonal mosaicism for chromosome alterations (mCA) and 226 males (5.08%) with acquired loss of chromosome Y (LOY). Individuals with clonal mosaic events (mCA and/or LOY) showed a 54% increase in the risk of COVID-19 lethality. LOY is associated with transcriptomic biomarkers of immune dysfunction, pro-coagulation activity and cardiovascular risk. Interferon-induced genes involved in the initial immune response to SARS-CoV-2 are also down-regulated in LOY. Thus, mCA and LOY underlie at least part of the sex-biased severity and mortality of COVID-19 in aging patients. Given its potential therapeutic and prognostic relevance, evaluation of clonal mosaicism should be implemented as biomarker of COVID-19 severity in elderly people. Among 9578 individuals diagnosed with COVID-19 in the SCOURGE study, individuals with clonal mosaic events (clonal mosaicism for chromosome alterations and/or loss of chromosome Y) showed an increased risk of COVID-19 lethality

    The Psychological Science Accelerator's COVID-19 rapid-response dataset

    Get PDF

    The psychological science accelerator’s COVID-19 rapid-response dataset

    Get PDF
    In response to the COVID-19 pandemic, the Psychological Science Accelerator coordinated three large-scale psychological studies to examine the effects of loss-gain framing, cognitive reappraisals, and autonomy framing manipulations on behavioral intentions and affective measures. The data collected (April to October 2020) included specific measures for each experimental study, a general questionnaire examining health prevention behaviors and COVID-19 experience, geographical and cultural context characterization, and demographic information for each participant. Each participant started the study with the same general questions and then was randomized to complete either one longer experiment or two shorter experiments. Data were provided by 73,223 participants with varying completion rates. Participants completed the survey from 111 geopolitical regions in 44 unique languages/dialects. The anonymized dataset described here is provided in both raw and processed formats to facilitate re-use and further analyses. The dataset offers secondary analytic opportunities to explore coping, framing, and self-determination across a diverse, global sample obtained at the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic, which can be merged with other time-sampled or geographic data

    A global experiment on motivating social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic

    Get PDF
    Finding communication strategies that effectively motivate social distancing continues to be a global public health priority during the COVID-19 pandemic. This cross-country, preregistered experiment (n = 25,718 from 89 countries) tested hypotheses concerning generalizable positive and negative outcomes of social distancing messages that promoted personal agency and reflective choices (i.e., an autonomy-supportive message) or were restrictive and shaming (i.e., a controlling message) compared with no message at all. Results partially supported experimental hypotheses in that the controlling message increased controlled motivation (a poorly internalized form of motivation relying on shame, guilt, and fear of social consequences) relative to no message. On the other hand, the autonomy-supportive message lowered feelings of defiance compared with the controlling message, but the controlling message did not differ from receiving no message at all. Unexpectedly, messages did not influence autonomous motivation (a highly internalized form of motivation relying on one’s core values) or behavioral intentions. Results supported hypothesized associations between people’s existing autonomous and controlled motivations and self-reported behavioral intentions to engage in social distancing. Controlled motivation was associated with more defiance and less long-term behavioral intention to engage in social distancing, whereas autonomous motivation was associated with less defiance and more short- and long-term intentions to social distance. Overall, this work highlights the potential harm of using shaming and pressuring language in public health communication, with implications for the current and future global health challenges

    To which world regions does the valence–dominance model of social perception apply?

    Get PDF
    Over the past 10 years, Oosterhof and Todorov’s valence–dominance model has emerged as the most prominent account of how people evaluate faces on social dimensions. In this model, two dimensions (valence and dominance) underpin social judgements of faces. Because this model has primarily been developed and tested in Western regions, it is unclear whether these findings apply to other regions. We addressed this question by replicating Oosterhof and Todorov’s methodology across 11 world regions, 41 countries and 11,570 participants. When we used Oosterhof and Todorov’s original analysis strategy, the valence–dominance model generalized across regions. When we used an alternative methodology to allow for correlated dimensions, we observed much less generalization. Collectively, these results suggest that, while the valence–dominance model generalizes very well across regions when dimensions are forced to be orthogonal, regional differences are revealed when we use different extraction methods and correlate and rotate the dimension reduction solution.C.L. was supported by the Vienna Science and Technology Fund (WWTF VRG13-007); L.M.D. was supported by ERC 647910 (KINSHIP); D.I.B. and N.I. received funding from CONICET, Argentina; L.K., F.K. and Á. Putz were supported by the European Social Fund (EFOP-3.6.1.-16-2016-00004; ‘Comprehensive Development for Implementing Smart Specialization Strategies at the University of PĂ©cs’). K.U. and E. Vergauwe were supported by a grant from the Swiss National Science Foundation (PZ00P1_154911 to E. Vergauwe). T.G. is supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC). M.A.V. was supported by grants 2016-T1/SOC-1395 (Comunidad de Madrid) and PSI2017-85159-P (AEI/FEDER UE). K.B. was supported by a grant from the National Science Centre, Poland (number 2015/19/D/HS6/00641). J. Bonick and J.W.L. were supported by the Joep Lange Institute. G.B. was supported by the Slovak Research and Development Agency (APVV-17-0418). H.I.J. and E.S. were supported by a French National Research Agency ‘Investissements d’Avenir’ programme grant (ANR-15-IDEX-02). T.D.G. was supported by an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship. The Raipur Group is thankful to: (1) the University Grants Commission, New Delhi, India for the research grants received through its SAP-DRS (Phase-III) scheme sanctioned to the School of Studies in Life Science; and (2) the Center for Translational Chronobiology at the School of Studies in Life Science, PRSU, Raipur, India for providing logistical support. K. Ask was supported by a small grant from the Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg. Y.Q. was supported by grants from the Beijing Natural Science Foundation (5184035) and CAS Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology. N.A.C. was supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (R010138018). We acknowledge the following research assistants: J. Muriithi and J. Ngugi (United States International University Africa); E. Adamo, D. Cafaro, V. Ciambrone, F. Dolce and E. Tolomeo (Magna GrĂŠcia University of Catanzaro); E. De Stefano (University of Padova); S. A. Escobar Abadia (University of Lincoln); L. E. Grimstad (Norwegian School of Economics (NHH)); L. C. Zamora (Franklin and Marshall College); R. E. Liang and R. C. Lo (Universiti Tunku Abdul Rahman); A. Short and L. Allen (Massey University, New Zealand), A. AteƟ, E. GĂŒneƟ and S. Can Özdemir (Boğaziçi University); I. Pedersen and T. Roos (Åbo Akademi University); N. Paetz (Escuela de ComunicaciĂłn MĂłnica Herrera); J. Green (University of Gothenburg); M. Krainz (University of Vienna, Austria); and B. Todorova (University of Vienna, Austria). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.https://www.nature.com/nathumbehav/am2023BiochemistryGeneticsMicrobiology and Plant Patholog
    • 

    corecore