7 research outputs found

    Patterns and potential mechanisms of thermal preference in E. muscae-infected Drosophila melanogaster

    Get PDF
    Animals use various strategies to defend against pathogens. Behavioral fever, or fighting infection by moving to warm locations, is seen in many ectotherms. The behavior-manipulating fungal pathogen Entomophthora muscae infects numerous dipterans, including fruit flies and house flies, Musca domestica. House flies have been shown to exhibit robust behavioral fever early after exposure to E. muscae, then switch to prefer cool temperatures in the later stages of infection. Interestingly, no evidence of behavioral fever in response to any investigated pathogen has been found in the fruit fly, Drosophila melanogaster. However, they have been found to prefer cool temperatures during infections. To determine if fruit flies utilize behavioral fever, cold-seeking, or both during E. muscae infection, we used a two-choice behavioral assay to measure individual temperature preferences of E. muscae-exposed and unexposed flies at early (24-72 hour) and late (72-120 hour) infection time points. In contrast with our expectation from house flies, fruit flies did not exhibit behavioral fever. However, we found significant cold temperature-seeking in flies that died from infection on the day of the assay. To investigate whether this cold-seeking behavior was being caused by the fly or the fungus, we tested the effects of temperature on the fitness of the host, D. melanogaster, and the pathogen, E. muscae, during infection. We found that flies held at low and high temperatures for 24 hours before death from infection laid no eggs at the lower temperature. This could suggest that the fly is not causing the cold-seeking behavior because there is no apparent fitness benefit at low temperatures. Conversely, cadavers sporulating at the low temperature tended to cause more flies to eventually die from infection, indicating that E. muscae infects flies more effectively at lower temperatures. Preliminarily, our results support fungal control of temperature preference before death, though further testing is needed. The idea that E. muscae benefits from colder temperatures, and therefore drives cold-seeking behavior in D. melanogaster at the end of the host’s life, expands our current knowledge about host behavior manipulation by E. muscae and provides a fascinating avenue for investigating the mechanisms by which this fungus manipulates complex behaviors in its animal host

    Variability in thermal and phototactic preferences in Drosophila may reflect an adaptive bet‐hedging strategy

    Get PDF
    Organisms use various strategies to cope with fluctuating environmental conditions. In diversified bet‐hedging, a single genotype exhibits phenotypic heterogeneity with the expectation that some individuals will survive transient selective pressures. To date, empirical evidence for bet‐hedging is scarce. Here, we observe that individual Drosophila melanogaster flies exhibit striking variation in light‐ and temperature‐preference behaviors. With a modeling approach that combines real world weather and climate data to simulate temperature preference‐dependent survival and reproduction, we find that a bet‐hedging strategy may underlie the observed interindividual behavioral diversity. Specifically, bet‐hedging outcompetes strategies in which individual thermal preferences are heritable. Animals employing bet‐hedging refrain from adapting to the coolness of spring with increased warm‐seeking that inevitably becomes counterproductive in the hot summer. This strategy is particularly valuable when mean seasonal temperatures are typical, or when there is considerable fluctuation in temperature within the season. The model predicts, and we experimentally verify, that the behaviors of individual flies are not heritable. Finally, we model the effects of historical weather data, climate change, and geographic seasonal variation on the optimal strategies underlying behavioral variation between individuals, characterizing the regimes in which bet‐hedging is advantageous

    Wild flies hedge their thermal preference bets in response to seasonal fluctuations

    Get PDF
    Fluctuating environmental pressures can challenge organisms by repeatedly shifting the optimum phenotype. Two contrasting evolutionary strategies to cope with these fluctuations are 1) evolution of the mean phenotype to follow the optimum (adaptive tracking) or 2) diversifying phenotypes so that at least some individuals have high fitness in the current fluctuation (bet-hedging). Bet-hedging could underlie stable differences in the behavior of individuals that are present even when genotype and environment are held constant. Instead of being simply ‘noise,’ behavioral variation across individuals may reflect an evolutionary strategy of phenotype diversification. Using geographically diverse wild-derived fly strains and high-throughput assays of individual preference, we tested whether thermal preference variation in Drosophila melanogaster could reflect a bet-hedging strategy. We also looked for evidence that populations from different regions differentially adopt bet-hedging or adaptive-tracking strategies. Computational modeling predicted regional differences in the relative advantage of bet-hedging, and we found patterns consistent with that in regional variation in thermal preference heritability. In addition, we found that temporal patterns in mean preference support bet-hedging predictions and that there is a genetic basis for thermal preference variability. Our empirical results point to bet-hedging in thermal preference as a potentially important evolutionary strategy in wild populations

    Wild flies hedge their thermal preference bets in response to seasonal fluctuations

    Get PDF
    Fluctuating environmental pressures can challenge organisms by repeatedly shifting the optimum phenotype. Two contrasting evolutionary strategies to cope with these fluctuations are 1) evolution of the mean phenotype to follow the optimum (adaptive tracking) or 2) diversifying phenotypes so that at least some individuals have high fitness in the current fluctuation (bet-hedging). Bet-hedging could underlie stable differences in the behavior of individuals that are present even when genotype and environment are held constant. Instead of being simply ‘noise,’ behavioral variation across individuals may reflect an evolutionary strategy of phenotype diversification. Using geographically diverse wild-derived fly strains and high-throughput assays of individual preference, we tested whether thermal preference variation in Drosophila melanogaster could reflect a bet-hedging strategy. We also looked for evidence that populations from different regions differentially adopt bet-hedging or adaptive-tracking strategies. Computational modeling predicted regional differences in the relative advantage of bet-hedging, and we found patterns consistent with that in regional variation in thermal preference heritability. In addition, we found that temporal patterns in mean preference support bet-hedging predictions and that there is a genetic basis for thermal preference variability. Our empirical results point to bet-hedging in thermal preference as a potentially important evolutionary strategy in wild populations
    corecore