84 research outputs found

    Content Differences in Syntactic and Semantic Representations

    Full text link
    Syntactic analysis plays an important role in semantic parsing, but the nature of this role remains a topic of ongoing debate. The debate has been constrained by the scarcity of empirical comparative studies between syntactic and semantic schemes, which hinders the development of parsing methods informed by the details of target schemes and constructions. We target this gap, and take Universal Dependencies (UD) and UCCA as a test case. After abstracting away from differences of convention or formalism, we find that most content divergences can be ascribed to: (1) UCCA's distinction between a Scene and a non-Scene; (2) UCCA's distinction between primary relations, secondary ones and participants; (3) different treatment of multi-word expressions, and (4) different treatment of inter-clause linkage. We further discuss the long tail of cases where the two schemes take markedly different approaches. Finally, we show that the proposed comparison methodology can be used for fine-grained evaluation of UCCA parsing, highlighting both challenges and potential sources for improvement. The substantial differences between the schemes suggest that semantic parsers are likely to benefit downstream text understanding applications beyond their syntactic counterparts.Comment: NAACL-HLT 2019 camera read

    The Role of Frequency in the Processing of giving and receiving Events in Korean

    Get PDF
    This study aimed to examine the processing benefits of frequency information associated with the case marker -eykey in comprehending Korean declarative sentences. By using a picture description task in which pictures ambiguously illustrated either a giving event (-eykeyREC 
 cwuta give 
 to) or a receiving event (-eykeySOURCE 
 patta receive 
 from), we found that giving events were predominantly preferred to receiving events. The results of the online sentence comprehension study revealed that 1) give-type verbs were integrated into sentences faster than receive-type verbs overall and 2) the reading-time differences between the verb types were significant when role NPs were canonically ordered (NP-eykey 
 NP-(l)ul) but not when they were noncanonically presented (NP-(l)ul 
 NP-eykey). We claim that structural and semantic frequency bias associated with -eykey facilitates readers anticipatory processing in the integration of upcoming information. We further discuss how the processing differences in giving and receiving events might attribute to the argument-adjunct distinction between recipients and sources

    Eesti keele kohakÀÀnded argumendistruktuuris

    Get PDF
    Keele rÀÀkijatena ei teadvusta me endale sageli, et kui me kasutame tegusĂ”nu, siis nendega kĂ€ivad kaasas teatud kÀÀnded, mis sĂ”ltuvad sellest tegusĂ”nast, mida kasutati. Vaatad telekat? SĂ”na „telekas“ ilmub osastavas kÀÀndes. Sööd pitsat vĂ”i sĂ”id pitsa Ă€ra? Osastav vĂ”i omastav kÀÀne. Unistad millestki? SeestĂŒtlev kÀÀne. Me teame, millist kÀÀnet kasutada, aga me ei tea, miks me seda teeme. Miks on tegusĂ”nade ja kÀÀnete sĂŒsteem selline nagu ta on ja miks kasutame mĂ”nede tegusĂ”nadega kohakÀÀndeid, kui nad ei tĂ€hista seal asukohti – nĂ€iteks „unistan pitsast“ vĂ”i „pidu jĂ€rgneb Ă”htusöögile“?TegusĂ”na kirjeldab sĂŒndmust vĂ”i seisundit, millel ĂŒldiselt on osalised. Fraase, mis vĂ€ljendavad neid osalisi, nimetame tegusĂ”na argumentideks. NĂ€iteks „sööma“ argumendid on see, kes sööb, ja see, mida sĂŒĂŒakse. Ilma nendeta poleks sel tegusĂ”nal tĂ€hendust. Nende kahe fraasi kÀÀnded sĂ”ltuvad tegusĂ”nast. Igal tegusĂ”nal on seega argumendistruktuur, ehk struktuur kindlate kÀÀnetega teatud konfiguratsioonis, mille abil saame teada, kes vĂ”i mis osales verbi kirjeldatud sĂŒndmuses ja millises rollis nad seda tegid. „Vaatama“ kasutab argumendistruktuuri nimetavas kÀÀndes vaatajaga ja osastavas kÀÀndes vaadatavaga. „Unistama“ kasutab aga struktuuri, kus unistaja on nimetavas ja unistamise objekt seestĂŒtlevas kÀÀndes.KĂ€esolev monograafia kirjeldab kuue kvantitatiivsete korpusuuringu ja ĂŒhe katse tulemusi, mille abil uuriti, milline roll on eesti keeles kohakÀÀnetega argumendistruktuuridel. Töö uurib kohakÀÀnetega struktuuride kolme aspekti – nendega ilmuvaid tegusĂ”nu, nendes ilmuvaid kÀÀndeid ja nende mĂ€rgitud argumendistaatuse tugevust.Esiteks, on pĂ”hjust arvata, et teatud struktuurid ilmuvad vaid teatud tĂŒĂŒpi tegusĂ”nadega, kuid pole selge, kuidas tĂ€pselt erinevad eesti keeles eri struktuuride tegusĂ”nad ĂŒksteisest. KĂ€esolev töö leidis, et kohakÀÀndestruktuuridega ilmuvad tegusĂ”nad viitavad palju staatilisematele oludele kui tavapĂ€rase osastava vĂ”i omastava struktuuriga tegusĂ”nad, kirjeldades pigem seisundeid kui sĂŒndmusi. Samuti leidis see, et enamjaolt ei ole kohakÀÀnetel sellises positsioonis enam kohatĂ€hendust, isegi mitte metafoorselt.Teiseks, töö uuris, kui grammatiseerunud on kuus eesti keele kohakÀÀnet. KĂ”rgem grammatiseerumistase viitab laialdasemale kasutusele argumendimarkerina ning kÀÀnde sagedasemale kasutusele kohatĂ€henduseta kontekstides. Töö leidis, et seestĂŒtlev ja alaleĂŒtlev kÀÀne on nii grammatiseerunud, et eesti keele rÀÀkijad kasutavad neid pigem ilma kohatĂ€henduseta, sealhulgas tĂ€histamaks sĂŒndmuste ja seisundite osalisi.Kolmandaks, monograafia keskendub argumendisideme tugevusele tegusĂ”na ja argumendi vahel. Keeleteaduses arvatakse, et grammatilises (nt osastavas) kÀÀndes argumendid („sööb leiba“) on tugevamad argumendid kui kohakÀÀndes argumendid („unistab leivast“). Esimest tĂŒĂŒpi argumente loetakse eesti keeles sihitiseks, aga teist tĂŒĂŒpi ei loeta. Töö jaoks lĂ€bi viidud katse nĂ€itab, et argumendiside on neis kahes kontekstis sama tugev ega sĂ”ltu kÀÀndest. Katse nĂ€itas ka, et argumendistaatus on oma olemuselt gradientne mitte binaarne, ning et mĂ”ned asukohafraasid on tegusĂ”nadega nii tugevalt seotud, et neid vĂ”ib lugeda poolargumentideks.KokkuvĂ”ttes annab vĂ€itekiri meile hulga uusi teadmisi sellest, kui lai vĂ”ib olla kohakÀÀnete kasutusala, millist tĂŒĂŒpi argumente nad markeerivad ning kuidas nad diakroonilises perspektiivis sellise funktsioonini jĂ”uavad. Töö tulemused on vĂ€gagi relevantsed keelteĂŒleses perspektiivis, arvestades morfoloogiliselt rikaste keelte (nt eesti keel) olulisust kÀÀnete ja nende sĂŒntaktilise funktsiooni uurimisel.Language speakers are not often aware of the fact that using certain verbs requires the use certain case affixes. The Estonian verb “vaatama“ (“watch“) requires its object to be in partitive case while “unistama” (“dream about”) takes an argument in elative case (“out of”). Speakers intuitively know which case to use, but they do not know what is behind this distrubution. What are the inner dynamics of the system around verbs and cases? How often and on what conditions are spatial cases (e.g. elative) used with verbs when they do not express spatial meaning, for instance “Ma unistan pitsa-st” (“I am dreaming about pizza”). Do they mark essentially the same type of argument status as grammatical cases (e.g. partitive)?Verbs express events or states that commonly have participants. Phrases referring to these participants are known as verbal arguments. For instance, “sööma” (“eat”) takes two arguments – the eater and the eaten. Without them, the verb would not have meaning. The cases on these two phrases depend on the verb. Each verb therefore has an argument structure, i.e. selects particular cases in a particular configuration, letting us know, what is involved in the event described by the verb, and in what type of role they play. “Vaatama” (“watch”) has a structure with a nominative subject and a partitive object. The structure of “unistama” (“dream about”), however, includes a nominative subject and an elative argument.This thesis presents six corpus studies and one experiment, all investigating Estonian argument structures with spatial cases. It focusses on the three main variables describing argument structures: the verbs with which they occur, the cases they include and the strength of argument status they mark.First, there is reason to think that different structures occur with different types of verbs. It is not clear, however, in what ways these verbs are distinct in Estonian. We found that verbs in spatial case argument structures (“unistama” – “dream about”) are more stative than verbs in more common argument structures (“sööma” – “eat”). We also found that in a wide range of verbs, spatial cases no longer have any trace of spatial meaning when marking their arguments.Second, we asked, which spatial cases are most grammaticalised. Cases on a higher grammaticalisation level mark arguments and occur without spatial meaning more frequently. We found that elative (“out of”) and allative (“onto”) are so grammaticalized that they are infrequently used for referring to space. Instead, they are used for talking about other types of meaning and relationships, including marking the highly abstract argument relation.Third, the thesis investigated argument status in various functions of spatial cases. Mainstream linguistic theory regards canonical objects (e.g. the object of “eat”) as stronger arguments than non-canonical arguments (e.g. the elative argument of “unistama” – “dream about”). Our experiment showed that spatial case structures include equally strong arguments as canonical structure, meaning case has little to do with argument status. It also demonstrated the gradient nature of argument status, outlining various types of semi-arguments.All in all, the thesis provides us with an abundance of new knowledge about how Estonian spatial cases function in the service of verbs. These results are highly relevant to cross-linguistic knowledge, given the important role morphologically rich languages such as Estonian play in studies investigating the use of morphology in marking syntactic relations.https://www.ester.ee/record=b552789

    Clitics, case, and word order in Yagua

    Get PDF

    Electrophysiological indices of syntactic processing difficulty

    Get PDF
    Thesis (Ph. D.)--Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Dept. of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, 1998.Includes bibliographical references (leaves [135]-[136]).Two types of processing difficulty are examined by means of electrical recordings taken from the scalp. One type of difficulty seems to be related to syntactic structural anomalies and another is related with memory load due to syntactic complexity. An experiment dealing with structural difficulty reveals the sensitivity of the parser with the argument status of the elements being processed. Memory constraints come into play when processing complex but structurally sound text strings. A number of experiments in this thesis examine a purported metric of complexity, namely, a left anterior negativity. It is argued that the predictive aspects of the parser is responsible for the complexity metric.by Anthony R. Harris.Ph.D
    • 

    corecore