81 research outputs found

    The Aggression and Vulnerablility Scale (AVS) for the measurement of inpatient behaviors: A demonstration of reliability

    Get PDF
    Background: The accurate and consistent recording of problematic incidents for in-patient behaviour is important in monitoring patient’s treatment and needs, and for comparing behaviour across different settings. Methods: The Aggression and Vulnerability Scale (AVS) classifies individual incidents into 10 categories covering aggression (Physical Aggression against Other People, Verbal Aggression, Aggression against Property, Non-Compliant Behaviour) and vulnerabilities (Self-Harm/Suicide Attempt, Self-Neglect, Victimized/Exploited, Sexual Vulnerability, Absconding Behaviour and Accident) and rates severity of the incident. Twenty raters (10 naΓ―ve and 10 experienced) rated 28 vignettes drawn from nursing records. Results: The AVS had high reliability for both classification category and severity in both experienced and inexperienced raters. Conclusion: The AVS is a reliable scheme for the coding of problematic behavior in an in-patient setting. The AVS is available free from the authors

    The effects of adapting to complex motions: position invariance and tuning to spiral motions

    Get PDF
    Recent neurophysiological evidence (e.g., Graziano, Andersen, & Snowden, 1994) suggested that some cells in the medial superior temporal area (MST) of the Old World monkey are sensitive to complex motions such as those brought about by a surface moving in depth or rotating. Two important findings were that these cells show position invariance (i.e., their preferred stimulus does not change across the receptive field), and that some cells were selective for β€œspiralling” stimuli rather than pure rotations or pure expansion/contractions. This paper attempts to provide evidence for similar processes in the human visual system by employing the technique of selective adaptation. We have simulated surfaces undergoing a motion in depth (div) or a rotation (curl), but have removed any cues that are not related to global motion. After adapting to a large pattern undergoing, say, an expansion, an aftereffect that contained an element of contraction could be elicited by placing small test patterns anywhere in the adapted area. This suggests that the global structure of the motion field must have been encoded as well as the local motion. Likewise thresholds for detecting motions similar to the adapting motion were elevated across the adapted area, while thresholds for other motions were not. Hence the effects of adaptation are both selective and show a degree of position invariance. Adaptation to pure div or pure curl stimuli was compared with adaptation to spiralling stimuli. Threshold elevation was always selective for the adapting motion and the shape and broadness of tuning did not vary. In simulations we could not reproduce our results using a model that had only div and curl detectors, but we could reproduce them if we allowed for detectors tuned for a broad range of spiral pitches. Our results suggest that humans encode the complex motion of surfaces by detectors tuned to many different types of motion and that the detectors are invariant across space in their properties

    The relationship between trauma and beliefs about hearing voices: a study of psychiatric and non-psychiatric voice hearers

    Get PDF
    Background Cognitive models suggest that distress associated with auditory hallucinations is best understood in terms of beliefs about voices. What is less clear is what factors govern such beliefs. This study aimed to explore the way in which traumatic life events contribute towards beliefs about voices and any associated distress. Method The difference in the nature and prevalence of traumatic life events and associated psychological sequelae was compared in two groups of voice hearers: psychiatric voice hearers with predominantly negative beliefs about voices (PVH) and non-psychiatric voice hearers with predominantly positive beliefs about voices (NPVH). The data from the two groups were then combined in order to examine which factors could significantly account for the variance in beliefs about voices and therefore levels of distress. Results Both groups reported a high prevalence of traumatic life events although significantly more PVH reported trauma symptoms sufficient for a diagnosis of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Furthermore, significantly more PVH reported experiencing childhood sexual abuse. Current trauma symptoms (re-experiencing, avoidance and hyperarousal) were found to be a significant predictor of beliefs about voices. Trauma variables accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in anxiety and depression. Conclusions The results suggest that beliefs about voices may be at least partially understood in the context of traumatic life events

    Response inhibition in the parametric Go/No-Go task in psychopathic offenders

    Get PDF
    Previous research on response inhibition in psychopaths has failed to find consistent evidence for aberrant inhibitory ability, despite strong expectations to the contrary. However, previous examinations have utilised inhibition paradigms that suffer from critical shortcomings, such as a lack of ecological validity and overly simplistic response criteria. To assess inhibition under conditions close to the demands of everyday settings, the current study employs a parametric Go/No-go task in male offenders (n77). Additionally, rather than treating psychopathy as a categorical descriptor, a dimensional approach is taken to assess the relationship between individual psychopathic traits and response inhibition performance. Results indicate significant relationships between response inhibition and individual facets of psychopathy as measured by the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version. A positive relationship was found between inhibitory ability and interpersonal aspects of psychopathy, reflecting an enhancement of inhibitory functioning for those scoring high on this facet. In addition, a negative association was found between psychopathic lifestyle characteristics and response inhibition. Whereas the negative association mirrors the conceptualisation of the lifestyle facet, the positive association between interpersonal psychopathic aspects and response inhibition might reflect a propensity for adaptive behaviour that enables psychopaths to adequately manipulate their victims and mask their true nature

    Actuarial prediction of violent recidivism in mentally disordered offenders

    Get PDF
    Background Actuarial instruments may be useful in predicting long-term violence in mentally disordered patients. We compared two instruments that differ in terms of what they are designed to predict (general versus violent recividism) and the inclusion of stable mental health variables. Method A large sample of mentally disordered patients were scored on two risk assessment instruments, the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide (VRAG) and the Offender Group Reconviction Scale (OGRS), based on information at the point of discharge. Their criminal histories for at least 2 years following discharge were obtained from official records. Results Both instruments were good predictors of both violent and general offending. Over shorter periods (0Β·85], which were significantly better than the OGRS. For longer follow-up periods the instruments had approximately equal prediction accuracy. However, both instruments predicted far more offences than were in fact recorded. Conclusions The VRAG is a very good predictor of future violence in the UK sample. The OGRS may also be of value as it can be completed quickly and without the need for mental health variables. Caution is needed, however, as both instruments appeared to over-predict the levels of reconvictions in this sample

    Аналіз ΠΊΡ–Π»ΡŒΠΊΡ–ΡΠ½ΠΈΡ… Ρ‚Π° якісних характСристик Π΄ΠΈΠ½Π°ΠΌΡ–ΠΊΠΈ Π½Π°ΡƒΠΊΠΎΠ²ΠΈΡ… ΠΊΠ°Π΄Ρ€Ρ–Π² Π²ΠΈΡ‰ΠΎΡ— ΠΊΠ²Π°Π»Ρ–Ρ„Ρ–ΠΊΠ°Ρ†Ρ–Ρ— Π£ΠΊΡ€Π°Ρ—Π½ΠΈ

    Get PDF
    ΠžΠ±Π³ΠΎΠ²ΠΎΡ€ΡŽΡ”Ρ‚ΡŒΡΡ ΠΏΡ€ΠΎΠ±Π»Π΅ΠΌΠ° ΠΊΡ–Π»ΡŒΠΊΡ–ΡΠ½ΠΈΡ… характСристик Ρ‚Π° Ρ–Π½Π΄ΠΈΠΊΠ°Ρ‚ΠΎΡ€Ρ–Π² якості Π½Π°ΡƒΠΊΠΎΠ²ΠΎ-ΠΊΠ°Π΄Ρ€ΠΎΠ²ΠΎΠ³ΠΎ ΠΏΠΎΡ‚Π΅Π½Ρ†Ρ–Π°Π»Ρƒ Π½Π°ΡƒΠΊΠΈ, ΠΌΠ΅Ρ‚ΠΎΠ΄ΠΈΠΊΠΈ Ρ—Ρ… ΠΎΡ†Ρ–Π½ΠΊΠΈ. НавСдСно ΠΊΡ–Π»ΡŒΠΊΡ–ΡΠ½Ρƒ ΠΎΡ†Ρ–Π½ΠΊΡƒ Π΄ΠΈΠ½Π°ΠΌΡ–ΠΊΠΈ Ρ‚Π° сучасного стану Π½Π°ΡƒΠΊΠΎΠ²ΠΎ-ΠΊΠ°Π΄Ρ€ΠΎΠ²ΠΎΠ³ΠΎ ΠΏΠΎΡ‚Π΅Π½Ρ†Ρ–Π°Π»Ρƒ Π²ΠΈΡ‰ΠΎΡ— ΠΊΠ²Π°Π»Ρ–Ρ„Ρ–ΠΊΠ°Ρ†Ρ–Ρ— Π£ΠΊΡ€Π°Ρ—Π½ΠΈ, ΠΎΡ†Ρ–Π½ΠΊΡƒ Π΄ΠΈΠ½Π°ΠΌΡ–ΠΊΠΈ Ρ‚Π° сучасного стану ΠΉΠΎΠ³ΠΎ якісних ΠΏΠΎΠΊΠ°Π·Π½ΠΈΠΊΡ–Π².ΠžΠ±ΡΡƒΠΆΠ΄Π°Π΅Ρ‚ΡΡ ΠΏΡ€ΠΎΠ±Π»Π΅ΠΌΠ° количСствСнных характСристик ΠΈ ΠΈΠ½Π΄ΠΈΠΊΠ°Ρ‚ΠΎΡ€ΠΎΠ² качСства Π½Π°ΡƒΡ‡Π½ΠΎ-ΠΊΠ°Π΄Ρ€ΠΎΠ²ΠΎΠ³ΠΎ ΠΏΠΎΡ‚Π΅Π½Ρ†ΠΈΠ°Π»Π° Π½Π°ΡƒΠΊΠΈ, ΠΌΠ΅Ρ‚ΠΎΠ΄ΠΈΠΊΠΈ ΠΈΡ… ΠΎΡ†Π΅Π½ΠΊΠΈ. ΠŸΡ€ΠΈΠ²Π΅Π΄Π΅Π½Ρ‹ количСствСнная ΠΎΡ†Π΅Π½ΠΊΠ° Π΄ΠΈΠ½Π°ΠΌΠΈΠΊΠΈ ΠΈ соврСмСнного состояния Π½Π°ΡƒΡ‡Π½ΠΎ -ΠΊΠ°Π΄Ρ€ΠΎΠ²ΠΎΠ³ΠΎ ΠΏΠΎΡ‚Π΅Π½Ρ†ΠΈΠ°Π»Π° Π²Ρ‹ΡΡˆΠ΅ΠΉ ΠΊΠ²Π°Π»ΠΈΡ„ΠΈΠΊΠ°Ρ†ΠΈΠΈ Π£ΠΊΡ€Π°ΠΈΠ½Ρ‹, ΠΎΡ†Π΅Π½ΠΊΠ° Π΄ΠΈΠ½Π°ΠΌΠΈΠΊΠΈ ΠΈ соврСмСнного состояния Π΅Π³ΠΎ качСствСнных ΠΏΠΎΠΊΠ°Π·Π°Ρ‚Π΅Π»Π΅ΠΉ.The discussion concerns the problem of quantitative parameters and indicators reflecting the quality of R&D personnel, and their measurement methodology. A quantitative measure is given of the dynamics, current conditions and qualitative indicators of the Ukrainian R&D personnel of the highest qualification

    Visual perception: Here's mud in your mind's eye

    Get PDF
    We appear to be unaware of large changes in our visual scene if our attention is temporarily diverted. This suggests that the rich, complete visual scene that we appear to have may be just an illusion

    Visual attention to color: parvocellular guidance of attentional resources?

    No full text
    Although transient changes in luminance have been well documented to automatically attract attention to their location, experiments looking at abrupt changes in color have failed to find similar attentional capture. These results are consistent with current theories of the role of the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) streams that postulate that the M stream, which is β€œcolor-blind,” plays the dominant role in guiding attention and eye movements. The experiment reported here used stimuli that contained only information defined by color, and masked residual luminance information with dynamic noise, to assess the capacity of purely chromatic cues to automatically guide spatial attention. Such stimuli were as effective as those containing large luminance signals in guiding attention. To the extent that these purely chromatic signals isolated the P stream, these results suggest that this stream is also capable of automatic attentional capture. Hence, color vision not only aids target identification but also is a strong aid for target detection and localization

    Shifts in perceived position following adaptation to visual motion

    Get PDF
    Where do we perceive an object to be when it is moving? Nijhawan [[1]] has reported that if a stationary test pattern is briefly flashed in spatial alignment with a moving one, the moving element actually appears displaced in the direction in which it is moving. Nijhawan postulates that this may be the result of a mechanism that predicts the future position of the moving element so as to compensate for the fact that the element will have moved position from the time at which the light left it to the time at which the observer becomes aware of it (as a result of the finite time taken for neural transmission). There is an alternative explanation of this effect, however. Changes in the stimulus presentation could affect perceptual latency [[2]], and therefore the perceived position if in motion (as suggested for the Pulfrich pendulum effect [[3,4]]). In other words, if the flashed probe of the Nijhawan demonstration takes longer to reach perceptual awareness than the moving stimulus, the latter will appear to be ahead of the probe. Here, I demonstrate an alternative way of testing this hypothesis. When an illusory movement is induced (via the motion aftereffect) within a stationary pattern, it can be shown that this also produces a change in its perceived spatial position. As the pattern is stationary, one cannot account for this result via the notion of perceptual lags

    Contrast gain mechanism or transient channel? Why the effects of a background pattern alter over time

    No full text
    If a brief test pulse is presented on a prolonged background pedestal, it is strongly masked if presented at the start of the pedestal, and is only weakly masked if presented 200 ms after the start. This has been suggested to occur due to contrast gain mechanisms that reduce the representation of the pedestal and therefore reduce its masking effects. We show here that the effect is due to the large transient in contrast that accompanies the onset of the pedestal. We find similar masking at pedestal offset, when the pedestal is continually flickered, or when pedestal and test have a high spatial frequency. These results were all predicted on the basis of sustained and transient channels
    • …
    corecore