7 research outputs found

    Data Analysis_Children_corrected SDT 20171109.xlsx

    No full text
    <p>We investigated perceptual discriminability for Asian-like versus Caucasian-like morph faces in school-age Taiwanese children and adults. One-hundred-and-two 5- to 12-year-old children and twenty-three adults performed a sequential same/different face matching task, where they viewed an Asian- or a Caucasian-parent face followed by either the same parent face or a different morphed face (containing 15%, 30%, 45%, or 60% contribution from the other parent face) and judged if the two faces looked the same. We computed the <i>dā€™</i> as the sensitivity index for each age groups. We also analyzed the group mean rejection rates as a function of the morph level and fitted with a cumulative normal distribution function. Results showed that the adults and the oldest 11-12-year-old children exhibited a greater sensitivity (<i>d</i>ā€™) and a smaller discrimination threshold (<i>m</i>) in the Asian-parent condition than those in the Caucasian-parent condition, indicating the presence of an own-race advantage. On the contrary, 5- to 10-year-old children showed an equal sensitivity and similar discrimination thresholds for both conditions, indicating an absence of the own-race advantage. Moreover, a gradual development in enhancing the discriminability for the Asian-parent condition was observed from age 5 to 12; however, the progression in the Caucasian-parent condition was less apparent. In sum, our findings suggest that expertise in face processing may take the entire childhood to develop, and supports the <i>perceptual learning view</i> of the other-race effect--the own-race advantage seen in adulthood likely reflects a result of prolonged learning specific to faces most commonly seen in oneā€™s visual environment such as own-race faces. </p

    Morphed female faces stimuli.

    No full text
    <p>An example of a set of linearly morphed female faces produced by image averaging of one Asian- and Caucasian-parent face pair. The numerator indicates the percent contribution to the morphed face from Asian parent and the denominator indicates the percent contribution from the Caucasian parent.</p

    Illustration of a sample trial of the sequential same/different face matching task.

    No full text
    <p>Illustration of a sample trial of the sequential same/different face matching task.</p

    The development of the own-race advantage in school-age children: A morphing face paradigm (adult's data)

    No full text
    We investigated perceptual discriminability for Asian-like versus Caucasian-like morph faces in school-age Taiwanese children and adults. One-hundred-and-two 5- to 12-year-old children and twenty-three adults performed a sequential same/different face matching task, where they viewed an Asian- or a Caucasian-parent face followed by either the same parent face or a different morphed face (containing 15%, 30%, 45%, or 60% contribution from the other parent face) and judged if the two faces looked the same. We computed the <i>dā€™</i> as the sensitivity index for each age groups. We also analyzed the group mean rejection rates as a function of the morph level and fitted with a cumulative normal distribution function. Results showed that the adults and the oldest 11-12-year-old children exhibited a greater sensitivity (<i>d</i>ā€™) and a smaller discrimination threshold (<i>m</i>) in the Asian-parent condition than those in the Caucasian-parent condition, indicating the presence of an own-race advantage. On the contrary, 5- to 10-year-old children showed an equal sensitivity and similar discrimination thresholds for both conditions, indicating an absence of the own-race advantage. Moreover, a gradual development in enhancing the discriminability for the Asian-parent condition was observed from age 5 to 12; however, the progression in the Caucasian-parent condition was less apparent. In sum, our findings suggest that expertise in face processing may take the entire childhood to develop, and supports the <i>perceptual learning view</i> of the other-race effect--the own-race advantage seen in adulthood likely reflects a result of prolonged learning specific to faces most commonly seen in oneā€™s visual environment such as own-race faces

    The estimated discrimination thresholds (<i>Ī¼</i>) and slope parameters (Ļƒ) of the group psychometric functions for the Asian-parent and Caucasian-parent conditions.

    No full text
    <p>The numbers in the parenthesis are standard errors (<i>SE</i>) of the estimates by <i>bootstrap resampling</i> with an iteration size of 200. Eleven iterations were performed for the adultsā€™ psychometric functions, and ten iterations were performed for the childrenā€™s functions).</p

    Adultsā€™ mean rejection rates.

    No full text
    <p>The adultsā€™ mean rejection rate (Y-axis) as a function of the morph level (X-axis) for the Asian-parent (black circles) and the Caucasian-parent condition (white circles). The rejection rate is the probability of judging the comparison face as ā€œdifferentā€ from the target face (i.e., the same as the <i>P</i>(diff) in <a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0195020#pone.0195020.t001" target="_blank">Table 1</a>). The morph level denotes the physical difference (in linear unit) between the target parent face and the comparison face.</p
    corecore