850 research outputs found
Risk Factors for Cognitive Decline
Cognitive impairment is one of the major symptoms of
dementia. The main cognitive functions acc orientation to time
and place, recall and memory, attention, language, calculation,
and visual construction. Impairment of cognitive functions influences
the ability of an individual to live independently, and it diminishes
the quality of life. In addition to the consequences for an individual,
cognitive impairment imposes a major burden on the health care
system because it induces an increased risk of institutionalization and
hospitalization.
Although cognlttve impairment is a less severe disorder than dementia,
it is much more common. In a representative papu lation of
subjects over 65 years of age, the prevalence of cognitive impairment
was 15.8%, whereas the prevalence of dementia was 4.2%.' The risk of
cognitive impairment rises exponentially with age. Therefore, we may
expect an increase in the number of people with cognitive impairment
in our aging society. At present, there acc a number of medications that
can delay the progression of dementia and that can stabilize cognitive
function. However, no cure or prevention for these disorders has been
found yet. Therefore, it is important to identify modifiable risk factors
for cognitive impairment and dementia. If these risk factors can be
found, preventive intervention may become feasible
No alignment of cattle along geomagnetic field lines found
This paper presents a study of the body orientation of domestic cattle on
free pastures in several European states, based on Google satellite
photographs. In sum, 232 herds with 3412 individuals were evaluated. Two
independent groups participated in our study and came to the same conclusion
that, in contradiction to the recent findings of other researchers, no
alignment of the animals and of their herds along geomagnetic field lines could
be found. Several possible reasons for this discrepancy should be taken into
account: poor quality of Google satellite photographs, difficulties in
determining the body axis, selection of herds or animals within herds, lack of
blinding in the evaluation, possible subconscious bias, and, most importantly,
high sensitivity of the calculated main directions of the Rayleigh vectors to
some kind of bias or to some overlooked or ignored confounder. This factor
could easily have led to an unsubstantiated positive conclusion about the
existence of magnetoreception.Comment: Added electronic supplement with source dat
A prospective study on circulating insulin-like growth factor I (IGF-I), IGF-binding proteins, and cognitive function in the elderly
The objective of this study was to investigate the longitudinal relation
between the insulin-like growth factor I (IGF-I)/IGF-binding protein
(IGFBP) system and cognitive function. The study population consisted of a
sample of 186 healthy participants from the population-based Rotterdam
Study, aged 55-80 yr. At baseline, we determined fasting blood levels of
free and total IGF-I, IGFBP-1, and IGFBP-3. The 30-point Mini-Mental State
Examination (MMSE) was used to assess cognitive impairment at baseline
(MMSE score of <26; 6% of the sample) and cognitive decline after, on the
average, 1.9 yr of follow-up (drop in MMSE score of >1 point/year; 22% of
the sample). Odds ratios (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) were
estimated using logistic regression, with adjustment for age, sex,
education, body mass index, and fasting insulin levels. Total IGF-I
appeared to be inversely related to cognitive impairment, although not
significantly. Higher total IGF-I and the total IGF-I/IGFBP-3 ratio were
associated with less cognitive decline (OR per SD increase = 0.65; 95% CI
= 0.44-0.95 and OR = 0.59; 95% CI = 0.39-0.87, respectively). No relation
was observed between free IGF-I and cognitive decline (OR = 0.99; 95% CI =
0.68-1.44). In conclusion, in this prospective study higher serum total
IGF-I levels and higher total IGF-I/IGFBP-3 ratios, but not higher free
IGF-I levels, were associated with less cognitive decline over the
following 2 yr. Circulating total IGF-I levels may reflect an underlying
biological process that influences cognitive decline
Smoking and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance at middle age.
In the elderly, cigarette smoking has been related to reduced cognitive performance and moderate alcohol consumption to increased cognitive performance. It is not clear whether these associations also exist in middle age. The authors examined these relations in a population-based cohort study of 1,927 randomly selected, predominantly middle-aged subjects aged 45-70 years at the time of cognitive testing and living in the Netherlands. From 1995 until 2000, an extensive cognitive battery was administered, and compound scores were calculated. Risk factors had been assessed approximately 5 years previously. Multiple linear regression analyses (in which one unit of the cognitive score = one standard deviation) showed that, after the authors adjusted for age, sex, education, alcohol consumption, and cardiovascular risk factors, current smokers had reduced psychomotor speed (beta = -0.159, 95% confidence interval: -0.071, -0.244; p = 0.0003) and reduced cognitive flexibility (beta = -0.133, 95% confidence interval: -0.035,-0.230; p = 0.008) compared with never smokers. This effect was similar to that of being approximately 4 years older. Alcohol consumption was related to increased speed and better flexibility, especially among women who drank 1-4 alcoholic beverages a day. In conclusion, among middle-aged subjects, current smoking was inversely and alcohol consumption positively related to psychomotor speed and cognitive flexibility. This finding suggests that actions to prevent cognitive decline can be taken in middle age
Glucose intolerance, hyperinsulinaemia and cognitive function in a general population of elderly men
Nonresponse of secondary respondents in multi-actor surveys: Determinants, consequences, and possible remedies
Multi-actor survey data are highly valuable for answering questions about family relations, but the collection of such data is complicated by nonresponse among secondary (nonresident) respondents. Little is known, however, about the degree to which nonresponse of secondary respondents is selective and about the degree to which selective nonresponse biases substantive findings. Using a large representative survey, we analyze nonresponse of nonresident adult children of primary respondents. Nonresponse appears strongly related to characteristics of the parent–child relationship and to characteristics of both parents and children. Consequences are examined for three dependent variables: children’s attitudes, children’s support giving to parents and children’s well-being. Heckman models, which correct for sample selection bias, show that selective response hardly biases the substantive estimates. keywords: values; intergenerational relations; well-being; non-response; survey methodolog
- …