6 research outputs found

    Example of a late pedestrian detection event in which the participant’s scotoma (Central Field Loss subject #4) was implicated.

    No full text
    <p>The upper graph depicts the pedestrian’s vertical position and size (black dots), the vertical extent and position of the scotoma at the horizontal position (magenta vertical lines), and the location of the vertical gaze Preferred Retinal Location (PRL) position (blue dots). Lower graph depicts horizontal position and gaze and the scotoma’s horizontal extent. When the pedestrian’s upper and lower limits are both within the vertical scotoma the pedestrian is not visible to the participant. Both the horizontal position of the pedestrian and the horizontal position and extent of the scotoma are relative to the car heading.</p

    Participant characteristics.

    No full text
    <p>* Mean ±Standard deviation [Range].</p><p>n/a = not applicable</p><p>Participant characteristics.</p

    Binocular visual field plots (PRL at origin), Reaction Time (RT) boxplots for Central Field Loss (CFL) subjects (24–26 pedestrian appearances at each eccentricity), and individual plots of RT by scotoma occlusion time.

    No full text
    <p>RT boxplot for normally-sighted controls is at bottom left. Box lengths show the 25% to 75% interquartile range (IQR) and whiskers show the maximum extent of cases that are not outliers (values >1.5 times IQR). Detection rates for each eccentricity are below each plot. CFL participant age, binocular contrast sensitivity (CS), binocular visual acuity and driver status are to the left of the plots. CFL5 had a vertical central scotoma and a paracentral lateral scotoma from OS scotoma overlapping OD physiological blind spot.</p

    Central Field Loss (CFL) and Normal Vision (NV) participant mean reaction times by drive type and hazard eccentricity.

    No full text
    <p>CFL participants had longer reaction times than controls at all eccentricities. Error bars represent 95% confidence interval.</p
    corecore