6 research outputs found

    Data_Sheet_1_The unembodied metaphor: comprehension and production of tactile metaphors without somatosensation.PDF

    No full text
    IntroductionProposals for embodied metaphor and embodied cognition have suggested abstract concepts are understood indirectly through the simulation of previous sensory experiences in a different domain. While exceptions have been observed for sensory deficits and impairments that are common, such as vision and audition, it is commonly assumed that somatosensation (proprioception, haptic touch, pain, pressure, temperature, etc.) is fundamental for the comprehension of production of sensory metaphors and much abstract thought in general. In this way, our past sensory experiences are critical to our understanding not just of the world around us but also of our sense of selves. This would suggest that Kim, who was born without somatosensation, would have difficulty understanding, using, or even thinking about many abstract concepts typically linked to different sensory experiences through metaphor, including a creation of a sense of self.MethodsTo examine her comprehension of sensory metaphors, Kim was asked to select the best sensory idiomatic expression given its context. Her friends and family as well as a representative sample of individuals online were recruited to complete the survey as controls. Additionally, we transcribed and analyzed six hours of unprompted speech to determine if Kim spontaneously uses somatosensory metaphors appropriately.ResultsResults from the idiomatic expression survey indicate that Kim performs as well as controls despite lacking any previous direct sensory experiences of these concepts. Analysis of the spontaneous speech highlights that Kim appropriately uses tactile expressions in both their concrete sensory and abstract metaphorical meanings.DiscussionTaken together, these two studies demonstrate that what is lost in sensory experiences can be made up in linguistic experiences, as Kim's understanding of tactile words was acquired in the complete absence of somatosensory experiences. This study demonstrates that individuals can comprehend and use tactile language and metaphor without recruiting past somatosensory experiences, and thus challenges a strong definition of embodied cognition which requires sensory simulations in language comprehension and abstract thought.</p

    Glucose challenge.

    No full text
    <p>(a) Weights after a 21 hour fast and prior to glucose challenge. (b) blood glucose levels at fasting baseline (timepoint 0) and following i.p. injection of dextrose (2 g/kg). Nβ€Š=β€Š6 (no sucrose) and 8 (unlimited).</p

    Concurrent choice.

    No full text
    <p>Average lever presses during (a) choice and (b) no choice conditions (nβ€Š=β€Š8). (c) Total food consumed (earned sucrose+freely available chow) during choice sessions (nβ€Š=β€Š8). (d) Percentage of total intake comprised of earned sucrose pellets during choice sessions (nβ€Š=β€Š8). Box plots: as described in <a href="http://www.plosone.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0003221#pone-0003221-g001" target="_blank">figure 1</a>; * p<.05; **p<.005; *** p<.0001.</p

    Experimental timeline, progressive ratio and activity.

    No full text
    <p>(a) Timeline indicating the order of experimental manipulations and tests. Two cohorts of mice were used and administered the experimental manipulation and testing procedures as indicated above and below the timeline. Breakpoint in progressive ratio (PR7) for the sucrose exposed and non-sucrose exposed mice tested under (b) food restriction and (c) sated conditions (nβ€Š=β€Š8). (d) Total distance traveled in the open field (1 hr) (nβ€Š=β€Š7). (e) Total number of wheel turns in 1 week (nβ€Š=β€Š7, 8). Box plots: Middle lines represent the median values, the top and bottom of the boxes represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, the whiskers represent the 10th and 90th percentiles and the dots represent mean values ; * pβ€Š=β€Š.05.</p

    High sugar/high fat dietary challenge.

    No full text
    <p>(a) Percent weight gain 3-weeks prior to (left) and during (right) the 3-week HS/HF exposure period in adulthood. (b) Body weight at the beginning and subsequent three weeks of HS/HF dietary options. (c) Metabolic efficiency as gram body weight increase per kcal consumed across the HS/HF dietary challenge. (d) Weekly consumption of standard chow (solid lines) and HS/HF options (dashed lines). Nβ€Š=β€Š5–7; Β±SEM, * p<.05.</p

    Sucrose preference.

    No full text
    <p>(a) Average preference expressed as sucrose consumed (g) divided by total sucrose and water consumption (g) (nβ€Š=β€Š7). Dashed line indicates no preference. (b) Average preference for the bottle previously paired with sucrose during extinction (nβ€Š=β€Š7). (c) Average sucrose and (d) water consumption (nβ€Š=β€Š7) Β±SEM; * p<.05, ** pβ€Š=β€Š.005.</p
    corecore