105 research outputs found

    Électrophysiologie des commotions cĂ©rĂ©brales chez les athlĂštes

    Full text link
    ThÚse numérisée par la Direction des bibliothÚques de l'Université de Montréal

    Quand l’action surpasse la perception : rĂŽle de la vision et de la proprioception dans la perception et le contrĂŽle en temps rĂ©el de l’orientation spatiale de la main

    Get PDF
    Cette recherche a pour but d’évaluer le rĂŽle de la vision et de la proprioception pour la perception et le contrĂŽle de l’orientation spatiale de la main chez l’humain. L’orientation spatiale de la main est une composante importante des mouvements d’atteinte et de saisie. Toutefois, peu d’attention a Ă©tĂ© portĂ©e Ă  l’étude de l’orientation spatiale de la main dans la littĂ©rature. À notre connaissance, cette Ă©tude est la premiĂšre Ă  Ă©valuer spĂ©cifiquement l’influence des informations sensorielles et de l’expĂ©rience visuelle pour la perception et le contrĂŽle en temps rĂ©el de l'orientation spatiale de la main pendant le mouvement d’atteinte naturel vers une cible stationnaire. Le premier objectif Ă©tait d’étudier la contribution de la vision et de la proprioception dans des tĂąches de perception et de mouvement d’orientation de la main. Dans la tĂąche de perception (orientation-matching task), les sujets devaient passivement ou activement aligner une poignĂ©e de forme rectangulaire avec une cible fixĂ©e dans diffĂ©rentes orientations. Les rotations de l’avant-bras et du poignet Ă©taient soit imposĂ©es par l’expĂ©rimentateur, soit effectuĂ©es par les sujets. Dans la tĂąche de mouvement d’orientation et d’atteinte simultanĂ©es (letter posting task 1), les sujets ont rĂ©alisĂ© des mouvements d’atteinte et de rotation simultanĂ©es de la main afin d’insĂ©rer la poignĂ©e rectangulaire dans une fente fixĂ©e dans les mĂȘmes orientations. Les tĂąches ont Ă©tĂ© rĂ©alisĂ©es dans diffĂ©rentes conditions sensorielles oĂč l’information visuelle de la cible et de la main Ă©tait manipulĂ©e. Dans la tĂąche perceptive, une augmentation des erreurs d’orientation de la main a Ă©tĂ© observĂ©e avec le retrait des informations visuelles concernant la cible et/ou ou la main. Lorsque la vision de la main n’était pas permise, il a gĂ©nĂ©ralement Ă©tĂ© observĂ© que les erreurs d’orientation de la main augmentaient avec le degrĂ© de rotation nĂ©cessaire pour aligner la main et la cible. Dans la tĂąche de mouvement d’orientation et d’atteinte simultanĂ©es, les erreurs ont Ă©galement augmentĂ© avec le retrait des informations visuelles. Toutefois, les patrons d’erreurs Ă©taient diffĂ©rents de ceux observĂ©s dans la tĂąche de perception, et les erreurs d’orientation n’ont pas augmentĂ© avec le degrĂ© de rotation nĂ©cessaire pour insĂ©rer la poignĂ©e dans la fente. En absence de vision de la main, il a Ă©tĂ© observĂ© que les erreurs d’orientation Ă©taient plus petites dans la tĂąche de mouvement que de perception, suggĂ©rant l’implication de la proprioception pour le contrĂŽle de l’orientation spatiale de la main lors des mouvements d’orientation et d’atteinte simultanĂ©es. Le deuxiĂšme objectif de cette recherche Ă©tait d’étudier l’influence de la vision et de la proprioception dans le contrĂŽle en temps rĂ©el de l’orientation spatiale de la main. Dans une tĂąche d’orientation de la main suivie d’une atteinte manuelle (letter posting task 2), les sujets devaient d’abord aligner l’orientation de la mĂȘme poignĂ©e avec la fente fixĂ©e dans les mĂȘmes orientations, puis rĂ©aliser un mouvement d’atteinte sans modifier l’orientation initiale de la main. Une augmentation des erreurs initiales et finales a Ă©tĂ© observĂ©e avec le retrait des informations visuelles. MalgrĂ© la consigne de ne pas changer l’orientation initiale de la main, une diminution des erreurs d’orientation a gĂ©nĂ©ralement Ă©tĂ© observĂ©e suite au mouvement d’atteinte, dans toutes les conditions sensorielles testĂ©es. Cette tendance n’a pas Ă©tĂ© observĂ©e lorsqu’aucune cible explicite n’était prĂ©sentĂ©e et que les sujets devaient conserver l’orientation de dĂ©part de la main pendant le mouvement d’atteinte (mouvement intransitif; letter-posting task 3). La diminution des erreurs pendant l’atteinte manuelle transitive vers une cible explicite (letter-posting task 2), malgrĂ© la consigne de ne pas changer l’orientation de la main pendant le mouvement, suggĂšre un mĂ©canisme de corrections automatiques pour le contrĂŽle en temps rĂ©el de l’orientation spatiale de la main pendant le mouvement d’atteinte naturel vers une cible stationnaire. Le troisiĂšme objectif de cette recherche Ă©tait d’évaluer la contribution de l’expĂ©rience visuelle pour la perception et le contrĂŽle de l’orientation spatiale de la main. Des sujets aveugles ont Ă©tĂ© testĂ©s dans les mĂȘmes tĂąches de perception et de mouvement. De maniĂšre gĂ©nĂ©rale, les sujets aveugles ont prĂ©sentĂ© les mĂȘmes tendances que les sujets voyants testĂ©s dans la condition proprioceptive (sans vision), suggĂ©rant que l’expĂ©rience visuelle n’est pas nĂ©cessaire pour le dĂ©veloppement d’un mĂ©canisme de correction en temps rĂ©el de l’orientation spatiale de la main basĂ© sur la proprioception.The goal of this research was to study the contribution of vision and proprioception to the perception and control of hand orientation in human subjects. Spatial orientation of the hand is an important component of reaching and grasping movements. However, not much attention has been given to spatial hand orientation in the literature. To our knowledge, this study is the first to specifically investigate the influence of sensory information for the perception and on-line control of hand orientation during natural reaching movement to stationary targets. The first objective of this research was to study the contribution of vision and proprioception in perceptual orientation-matching and motor letter posting tasks. In the perceptual orientation-matching task, subjects attempted to passively or actively align a match handle, to a target that was fixed in different orientations. In the passive perceptual task, passive rotations of the forearm and wrist were imposed by the experimenter; whereas in the active perceptual task, the rotations were actively executed by the subjects. In letter posting task 1, subjects simultaneously reached and rotated the right hand to insert a match handle into a target slot fixed in the same orientations. The tasks were performed in different sensory conditions where the visual information about the target and the hand was manipulated. In the perceptual orientation-matching task, augmentation of hand orientation errors was observed with the withdrawal of visual information related to either the target and/or the hand. When full vision was not allowed, hand orientation errors were larger overall when larger rotations of the wrist were required to match the target, whether the rotations were made actively by the subject or were imposed passively by the experimenter. In letter posting task 1, augmentation of hand orientation errors was also observed with the withdrawal of visual information related to either the target and/or the hand. However, errors patterns were different from those observed in the perceptual task, and hand orientation errors were not larger for larger target orientations. Without vision of the hand, final hand orientation errors were smaller overall in letter-posting task 1 than in the orientation-matching task. This suggests the implication of the proprioceptive information for the control of spatial hand orientation during reach-and-orient movements. The second objective of this research was to study the influence of vision and proprioception in on-line control of spatial hand orientation. In letter posting task 2, subjects first aligned their hand to the angle of the target and then reached to it with the instruction not to change their initial hand orientation. The augmentation of initial and final errors was observed with the withdrawal of vision. Although subjects were instructed to not change their hand orientation, in all sensory condition tested, hand orientation changed overall during reaching in a way that reduced the initial orientation errors. This trend did not occur when there was no explicitly defined target toward which the subjects reached (letter-posting task 3; intransitive movement). The reduction in hand orientation errors during transitive reach in letter-posting task 2, even when told not to change it, suggests the engagement of an automatic error correction mechanism for hand orientation during natural reaching movements toward stationary targets. The third objective of this research was to investigate the contribution of visual experience to the perception and control of spatial orientation of the hand. Blind subjects were tested in the same perceptual and motor tasks. Overall, no differences were observed between performance of blind subjects and normally-sighted subjects tested without vision (proprioceptive condition), suggesting that prior visual experience is not necessary for the development of an on-line error correction mechanism for hand orientation guided by proprioceptive inputs

    Main features of the timber structure building industry business models

    Get PDF
    The use of timber as structural building material is growing and a greater number of firms are looking to enter this raising market. Erecting a complex timber building usually involves combining the work of architects, structural engineers, builders, suppliers and/or supplier–builders, all of them having their own business models. The purpose of this research was to uncover the specific nature of business models in the timber structure building industry. First, a thorough mapping of these business models was undertaken. Second, underlying patterns were uncovered within these models. A triangulation method of secondary data, semi-structured interviews and participant observation was used to allow for an in-depth study of 23 stakeholder business models. The analysis shows that knowledge sharing appears as crucial and may be achieved through sustained collaboration. As a result, collaborative contract procurement modes seem to be the most appropriate for timber construction. Tight relationships with suppliers and supplier–builders also appear as prerequisites. Furthermore, stakeholder partnerships with universities appear common in the field, while prefabrication is increasing in popularity. These findings can be useful to grasp the prevailing business models in this industry given the sustained growth of the timber structure building market

    Driving Difficulties and Adaptive Strategies: The Perception of Individuals Having Sustained a Mild Traumatic Brain Injury

    Get PDF
    Introduction. After a mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI), individuals quickly resume driving. However, relatively little is known about the impact of mTBI on driving ability and, notably, on the perceived influence of postconcussive symptoms on driving. Hence, the objective of this study was to document the perception of driving abilities in individuals with mTBI. Method. Twenty-seven drivers with mTBI were interviewed to document their perception regarding their driving abilities. Both driving-related difficulties and compensatory strategies used to increase driving safety were documented. A mixed quantitative and qualitative analysis of the data was completed. Results. 93% of participants reported at least one difficulty perceived as having an impact on everyday activities. Most frequently named problems affecting driving were fatigue and reduced concentration. In addition, 74% of participants had adapted their driving or developed strategies to compensate for driving difficulties. Discussion/Conclusion. Postconcussive symptoms have repercussions on driving ability. However, people with mTBI tend to be aware of their difficulties and develop, over time, adaptive strategies. Preventive measures are thus warranted to increase health care professionals' awareness of the potential consequences of mTBI on driving ability and to promote guidelines for the safe resumption of driving after injury

    A longitudinal investigation of sleep and daytime wakefulness in children and youth with concussion

    Get PDF
    A high proportion of adults who sustain a concussion identify changes in their sleep during the acute stage, typically reporting an increased need for sleep or nonrestful sleep. Our understanding of sleep following concussion is less well understood within a pediatric population. In this study, we investigated the trajectory of sleep and daytime sleepiness in a prospective cohort of 40 children and youth (6–18 years old) with concussion, 40 age-and sex-matched healthy children and youth, and 40 with upper-extremity orthopedic injury. Evaluations occurred during the acute stage (<2 weeks) and at 3-, 6-, and 12-month postinjury using the Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children and the Postconcussion Symptom Scale. There were no significant differences within- or between-group differences in sleep across all four time points with analysis of the groups as a whole. When groups were divided by age (6–11 and 12– < 18 years), there was a significant difference in the ability to initiate sleep for the younger concussed group during the acute stage, compared with healthy controls, as well as significantly greater daytime nap duration that decreased over time. Significant correlations were also found between the frequency and duration of daytime naps and Postconcussion Symptom Scale total score and subscores (cognitive, physical/migraine, mood, and sleep) in the concussed group during the acute stage. Our results suggest that in a group with noncomplicated concussion, children and youth have transient alterations in daytime sleepiness that are related to concussion symptoms. Younger children may be more vulnerable to disturbances in sleep and daytime wakefulness

    The impact of poor sleep on cognition and activities of daily living after traumatic brain injury : a review

    Full text link
    Background/aim : Patients frequently report sleep disrup-tions or insomnia during their hospital stay, particularlyafter a traumatic brain injury (TBI). The consequences ofthese sleep disturbances on everyday activities are not welldocumented and are therefore not considered in the evalu-ation of independence in activities of daily living (ADLs).The goal of this narrative review is to explore the conse-quences of poor sleep quality on cognition and ADLs inthe acute and subacute stages of a moderate and severeTBI, when patients are in acute care or inpatient rehabili-tation.Methods:We will present an overview of normal sleepand its role in cognitive functioning, and then present thefindings of studies that have investigated sleep characteris-tics in hospital settings and the consequences of sleep dis-turbances on ADLs.Results:During hospitalisation, TBI patients presentsevere sleep disturbances such as insomnia and sleepfragmentation, which are probably influenced by both themedical condition and the hospital or rehabilitation environ-ment. Sleep disruption is associated with several cognitivedeficits, including attention, memory and executive func-tion impairments. Poor quality and/or insufficient quantityof sleep in acute TBI probably affect general functioningand ADLs calling for these cognitive functions.Conclusions and Significance:The cognitive impair-ments present following TBI are probably exacerbated bypoor sleep quality and sleep deprivation during hospitali-sation, which in turn impact ADLs among this popula-tion. Health-care personnel should further consider sleepdisturbances among people with TBI and a sleep protocolshould be established

    Evolution of severe sleep-wake cycle disturbances following traumatic brain injury: a case study in both acute and subacute phases post-injury

    Get PDF
    Abstract Background Sleep-wake disturbances are frequently reported following traumatic brain injury (TBI), but they remain poorly documented in the acute stage of injury. Little is known about their origin and evolution. Case presentation This study presents the case of a patient in the acute phase of a severe TBI. The patient was injured at work when falling 12 m into a mine and was hospitalized in the regular wards of a level I trauma centre. From days 31 to 45 post-injury, once he had reached a level of medical stability and continuous analgosedation had been ceased, his sleep-wake cycle was monitored using actigraphy. Results showed significant sleep-wake disturbances and severe sleep deprivation. Indeed, the patient had an average nighttime sleep efficiency of 32.7 ± 15.4 %, and only an average of 4.8 ± 1.3 h of sleep per 24-h period. After hospital discharge to the rehabilitation centre, where he remained for 5 days, the patient was readmitted to the same neurological unit for paranoid delusions. During his second hospital stay, actigraphy recordings resumed from days 69 to 75 post-injury. A major improvement in his sleep-wake cycle was observed during this second stay, with an average nighttime sleep efficiency of 96.3 ± 0.9 % and an average of 14.1 ± 0.9 h of sleep per 24-h period. Conclusion This study is the first to extensively document sleep-wake disturbances in both the acute and subacute phases of severe TBI. Results show that prolonged sleep deprivation can be observed after TBI, and suggest that the hospital environment only partially contributes to sleep-wake disturbances. Continuous actigraphic monitoring may prove to be a useful clinical tool in the monitoring of patients hospitalized after severe TBI in order to detect severe sleep deprivation requiring intervention. The direct impact of sleep-wake disturbances on physiological and cognitive recovery is not well understood within this population, but is worth investigating and improving

    EEG functional connectivity prior to sleepwalking : evidence of interplay between sleep and wakefulness

    Full text link
    Study Objectives: Although sleepwalking (somnambulism) affects up to 4% of adults, its pathophysiology remains poorly understood. Sleepwalking can be preceded by fluctuations in slow-wave sleep EEG signals, but the significance of these pre-episode changes remains unknown and methods based on EEG functional connectivity have yet to be used to better comprehend the disorder. Methods: We investigated the sleep EEG of 27 adult sleepwalkers (mean age: 29 ± 7.6 years) who experienced a somnambulistic episode during slow-wave sleep. The 20-second segment of sleep EEG immediately preceding each patient’s episode was compared with the 20-second segment occurring 2 minutes prior to episode onset. Results: Results from spectral analyses revealed increased delta and theta spectral power in the 20 seconds preceding the episodes’ onset as compared to the 20 seconds occurring 2 minutes before the episodes. The imaginary part of the coherence immediately prior to episode onset revealed (1) decreased delta EEG functional connectivity in parietal and occipital regions, (2) increased alpha connectivity over a fronto-parietal network, and (3) increased beta connectivity involving symmetric inter-hemispheric networks implicating frontotemporal, parietal and occipital areas. Conclusions: Taken together, these modifications in EEG functional connectivity suggest that somnambulistic episodes are preceded by brain processes characterized by the co-existence of arousal and deep slee

    Towards a better understanding of increased sleep duration in the chronic phase of moderate to severe traumatic brain injury : an actigraphy study

    Full text link
    Introduction Most adults with moderate to severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) report persistent sleep-wake disturbances. Whether these complaints are either associated with abnormal sleep-wake patterns or can be explained by TBI-related characteristics is unclear. The present study aimed at characterising the subjective and objective sleep-wake patterns in TBI adults by taking into consideration the influence of TBI severity, common comorbidities and psychoactive medication. Methods Overall, 34 adults with moderate-severe TBI (one to four years post-injury) were compared to 34 controls. Sleepiness, fatigue, sleep quality, mood, and pain were assessed with questionnaires. A seven day sleep diary and actigraphy was used to document sleep and wake patterns. Results Compared to controls, TBI participants reported more sleepiness and fatigue, as well as poorer sleep quality. On actigraphy, they had earlier bedtime and longer time spent in bed, but equivalent sleep efficiency during the nighttime episode compared to controls. TBI participants also took more naps and accumulated more time asleep over the 24 h period than controls. These group differences were accentuated when only TBI adults using psychoactive medication were included. More comorbidities, more severe injuries and longer hospital stay were positively correlated with fatigue, sleepiness and sleep duration. Conclusions Our results showed that despite complaints regarding sleep and diurnal functioning, TBI survivors have very marginal changes in their objective sleep-wake schedules. Prolonged time spent in bed may reflect an attempt to increase their sleep duration in response to fatigue and sleepiness. TBI adults who use psychoactive medication are those with more evident changes in their sleep-wake schedules

    Waking EEG functional connectivity in middle-aged and older adults with obstructive sleep apnea

    Full text link
    Objectives: The present study aimed at investigating changes in waking electroencephalography (EEG), most specifically regarding spectral power and functional connectivity, in middle-aged and older adults with OSA. We also explored whether changes in spectral power or functional connectivity are associated with polysomnographic characteristics and/or neuropsychological performance. Methods: 19 OSA subjects (apnea-hypopnea index ≄ 20, age: 63.6 ± 6.4) and 22 controls (apneahypopnea index ≀ 10, age: 63.6 ± 6.7) underwent a full night of in-laboratory polysomnography followed by a waking EEG and a neuropsychological assessment. Waking EEG spectral power and imaginary coherence were compared between groups for all EEG frequency bands and scalp regions. Correlation analyses were performed between selected waking EEG variables, polysomnographic parameters and neuropsychological performance. Results: No group difference was observed for EEG spectral power for any frequency band. Regarding the imaginary coherence, when compared to controls, OSA subjects showed decreased EEG connectivity between frontal and temporal regions in theta and alpha bands as well as increased connectivity between frontal and parietal regions in delta and beta 1 bands. In the OSA group, these changes in connectivity correlated with lower sleep efficiency, lower total sleep time and higher apnea-hypopnea index. No relationship was found with neuropsychological performance. Conclusions: Contrary to spectral power, imaginary coherence was sensitive enough to detect changes in brain function in middle-aged and older subjects with OSA when compared to controls. Whether these changes in cerebral connectivity predict cognitive decline needs to be investigated longitudinally
    • 

    corecore