79 research outputs found

    Interpreting the Paradoxical in the Hispanic Paradox

    Full text link
    This paper discusses problems that are common to both the epidemiologic risk-factor approach and the demographic variable-based approach to studying population health. We argue that there is a shared reluctance to move away from a narrow variable-based thinking that pervades both disciplines, and a tendency to reify the multivariate linear procedures employed in both disciplines. In particular, we concentrate on the difficulties generated by classical variable-based approaches that are especially striking when one neglects selection processes and the use of strategies to minimize its effects. We illustrate these difficulties in terms of the so-called “Hispanic Paradox”, which refers to comparative health advantages that some Hispanic groups appear to have. We find that much of what is conceived by demographers and epidemiologists as a paradox may not be paradoxical at all.Peer Reviewedhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/73920/1/j.1749-6632.2001.tb02751.x.pd

    Mujahid et al. Respond to "Beyond the Metrics for Measuring Neighborhood Effects"

    Full text link
    In her commentary, Dr. Lynne Messer (1) recognizes the important contributions of our paper (2) to the discussion of methodological issues related to measurement of neighborhood or area-level properties. Dr. Messer reviews the many challenges involved in observational studies of neighborhood health effects, which we and other investigators have noted (3–8). A major challenge is developing theoretical models of the processes through which neighborhoods (or areas) may affect health. Messer argues that our paper "promises more, from a theoretical perspective, than it delivers" (1, p. 869). Our paper is merely a methodological illustration, with no grandiose theoretical aims. However, we do base the measures we explore on a theoretical model of the processes through which residential context may affect cardiovascular disease risk (1, 9). In her discussion of this model, Messer confuses inconsistent empirical support for aspects of the model with the absence of theory itself. Theorizing on the spatial scale at which different area processes operate is obviously important, but unfortunately there is very little information on which to base this theory. Additional qualitative research on the ways in which individuals interact with spaces may help us develop better theoretical models that may then be empirically tested. However, even if we were able to offer some crude hypotheses regarding spatial scales relevant to different processes, there are features of areas that could plausibly operate at multiple levels. Ultimately, we must rely on empirical research to uncover such relations rather than make a priori assertions under the guise of theory. For this, improving the validity of area-level measures and sensitivity analyses like the ones we present is crucial. Dr. Messer also alludes to the well-established challenges in estimating causal effects from observational data. Nonexchangeability (or its simpler and less fashionable synonym, "residual confounding") is always a concern. Messer implies that because of this, observational work in neighborhood health-effects research is meaningless. Firm believers in nonexchangeability will accept no defense of observational studies because it is impossible to categorically rule out residual confounding, except in the case of the ideal counterfactual experiment. However, claims of residual confounding also need to be subjected to empirical inquiry: What specific confounders have been omitted, and how strong are their effects expected to be? Careful observational work can empirically examine the sensitivity of results to different degrees of residual confounding and degrees of extrapolation. In this, neighborhood effects research is no different than the rest of epidemiology. Given the many limitations and logistical challenges of randomized trials (particularly for the study of neighborhood effects), reliance on observational and quasi-experimental data is likely to continue. Hence, anything we can do to improve the rigor of observational work is crucial. Our objective in the current paper was (merely) to contribute to emerging work on the measurement of area-level constructs, not to fully develop a theory on neighborhood causal effects or to resolve the issue of relevant spatial scale. Our objective was not even to estimate associations between neighborhood characteristics and health outcomes. Instead, we wanted to further develop and evaluate our ability to measure area-level constructs. Epidemiologists are very sophisticated at measuring individual-level characteristics but not as sophisticated at measuring features of ecologic settings. This seriously hampers their ability to examine contextual effects. Our analyses illustrate one approach to quantifying the measurement properties of area-based measures. This approach can be adapted to different constructs and different spatial scales, depending on the research problem and underlying theory. We firmly believe that improving the quality of measurement of area-level constructs is a prerequisite for more rigorous observational work. In fact, several of the inferential problems that arise when area socioeconomic status characteristics are used as proxies for features of areas may be reduced when specific features of areas are examined instead of aggregate socioeconomic status measures (which are, by definition, correlated with individual socioeconomic status, thus magnifying the extrapolation and exchangeability problems). We hope that the illustration we provide in our paper (2) will encourage other investigators to develop and test theoretically relevant area measures and to contrast different approaches to their measurement. Understanding if and how contexts (including neighborhoods) affect health is challenging and complex, but it is also enormously important from the point of view of public health and policy. In order to answer questions regarding these effects, we need to move beyond blanket (and sometimes facile) critiques, roll up our sleeves, and see if we can improve on the work that has been done to date. This means dealing with a messy, correlated, and confounded reality and doing the best we can to glean truth from our observations. As epidemiologists, this is our job, and also our responsibility to the public.http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/58002/1/Mujahid et al Respond to Beyond the Metrics for Measuring Neighborhood Effects.pd

    Understanding social disparities in hypertension prevalence, awareness, treatment, and control: The role of neighborhood context.

    Full text link
    Peer Reviewedhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/57187/1/Morenoff JD et al 2007 Understanding social disparities in hypertension prevalence awareness treatment and control The role of neighborhood context.pd

    Neighborhood Characteristics and Hypertension

    Full text link
    Background: The goal of this study was to investigate cross-sectional associations between features of neighborhoods and hypertension and to examine the sensitivity of results to various methods of estimating neighborhood conditions. Methods: We used data from the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis on 2612 individuals 45–85 years of age. Hypertension was defined as systolic blood pressure above 140 mm Hg, diastolic pressure above 90 mm Hg, or use of antihypertensive medications. Neighborhood (census tract) conditions potentially related to hypertension (walking environment, availability of healthy foods, safety, social cohesion) were measured using information from a separate phone survey conducted in the study neighborhoods. For each neighborhood we estimated scale scores by aggregating residents’ responses using simple aggregation (crude means) and empirical Bayes estimation (unconditional, conditional, and spatial). These estimates of neighborhood conditions were linked to each study participant based on the census tract of residence. Two-level binomial regression methods were used to estimate adjusted associations between neighborhood conditions and hypertension. Results: Residents of neighborhoods with better walkability, availability of healthy foods, greater safety, and more social cohesion were less likely to be hypertensive (relative prevalence [95% confidence interval] for 90th vs. 10th percentile of conditional empirical Bayes estimate = 0.75 [0.64–0.88], 0.72 [0.61–0.85], 0.74 [0.63–0.86], and 0.69 [0.57–0.83]), respectively, after adjusting for site, age, sex, income, and education. Associations were attenuated and often disappeared after additional adjustments for race/ethnicity. Conclusion: Neighborhood walkability, food availability, safety, and social cohesion may be mechanisms that link neighborhoods to hypertension.http://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/60338/1/Neighborhood Characteristics and Hypertension.pd

    Incarceration, Prisoner Reentry, and Communities.

    No full text

    Incarceration, Prisoner Reentry, and Communities.

    No full text
    Since the mid-1970s the United States has experienced an enormous rise in incarceration and accompanying increases in returning prisoners and in post-release community correctional supervision. Poor urban communities are disproportionately impacted by these phenomena. This review focuses on two complementary questions regarding incarceration, prisoner reentry, and communities:(1) whether and how mass incarceration has affected the social and economic structure of American communities, and (2) how residential neighborhoods affect the social and economic reintegration of returning prisoners. These two questions can be seen as part of a dynamic process involving a pernicious "feedback" loop, in which mass incarceration undermines the structure and social organization of some communities, thus creating more criminogenic environments for returning prisoners and further diminishing their prospects for successful reentry and reintegration

    Social Anatomy of Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Violence

    No full text
    We analyzed key individual, family, and neighborhood factors to assess competing hypotheses regarding racial/ethnic gaps in perpetrating violence. From 1995 to 2002, we collected 3 waves of data on 2974 participants aged 18 to 25 years living in 180 Chicago neighborhoods, augmented by a separate community survey of 8782 Chicago residents. The odds of perpetrating violence were 85% higher for Blacks compared with Whites, whereas Latino-perpetrated violence was 10% lower. Yet the majority of the Black–White gap (over 60%) and the entire Latino–White gap were explained primarily by the marital status of parents, immigrant generation, and dimensions of neighborhood social context. The results imply that generic interventions to improve neighborhood conditions and support families may reduce racial gaps in violence
    • …
    corecore