30 research outputs found
Health Plan Choice and Information about Out-of-Pocket Costs: An Experimental Analysis
Many consumers are offered two or more employer-sponsored health insurance plans, and competition among health plans for subscribers is promoted as a mechanism for balancing health care costs and quality. Yet consumers may not receive the information necessary to make informed health plan choices. This study tests the effects on health plan choice of providing supplemental decision-support materials to inform consumers about expected health plan costs. Our main finding is that such information induces consumers to bear more risk, especially those in relatively good health. Thus our results suggest that working-age, privately insured consumers currently may be over-insuring for medical care
Are medical treatments for individuals and groups like single-play and multiple-play gambles?
People are often more likely to accept risky monetary gambles with positive expected values when the gambles will be played more than once. We investigated whether this distinction between single-play and multiple-play gambles extends to medical treatments for individual patients and groups of patients. Resident physicians and medical students (extit{n} = 69) and undergraduates (extit{n} = 99) ranked 9 different flu shots and a no-flu-shot option in 1 of 4 combinations of perspective (individual patient vs. group of 1000 patients) and uncertainty frame (probability vs. frequency). The rank of the no-flu-shot option (a measure of preference for treatment vs. no treatment) was not significantly related to perspective or participant population. The main effect of uncertainty frame and the interaction between perspective and uncertainty frame approached significance (0.1 {extgreater} extit{p} {extgreater} 0.05), with the no-flu-shot option faring particularly poorly (treatment faring particularly well) when decisions about many patients were based on frequency information. Undergraduate participants believed that the no-flu-shot option would be less attractive (treatment would be more attractive) in decisions about many patients, but these intuitions were inconsistent with the actual ranks. These results and those of other studies suggest that medical treatments for individuals and groups are not analogous to single-play and multiple-play monetary gambles, perhaps because many people are unwilling to aggregate treatment outcomes over patients in the same way that they would compute net gains or losses over monetary gambles
Recommended from our members
Taxes on Sugar-Sweetened Beverages to Reduce Overweight and Obesity in Middle-Income Countries: A Systematic Review
Background: The consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs), which can lead to weight gain, is rising in middle-income countries (MICs). Taxing SSBs may help address this challenge. Systematic reviews focused on high-income countries indicate that taxing SSBs may reduce SSB consumption. Responsiveness to price changes may differ in MICs, where governments are considering the tax. To help inform their policy decisions, this review compiles evidence from MICs, assessing post-tax price increases (objective 1), changes in demand for SSBs and other products, overall and by socio-economic groups (objective 2), and effects on overweight and obesity prevalence (objective 3). Methods and Findings: We conducted a systematic review on the effectiveness of SSB taxation in MICs (1990–2016) and identified nine studies from Brazil, Ecuador, India, Mexico, Peru, and South Africa. Estimates for own-price elasticity ranged from -0.6 to -1.2, and decreases in SSB consumption ranged from 5 to 39 kilojoules per person per day given a 10% increase in SSB prices. The review found that milk is a likely substitute, and foods prepared away from home, snacks, and candy are likely complements to SSBs. A quasi-experimental study and two modeling studies also found a negative relationship between SSB prices and obesity outcomes after accounting for substitution effects. Estimates are consistent despite variation in baseline obesity prevalence and per person per day consumption of SSBs across countries studied. Conclusions: The review indicates that taxing SSBs will increase the prices of SSBs, especially sugary soda, in markets with few producers. Taxing SSBs will also reduce net energy intake by enough to prevent further growth in obesity prevalence, but not to reduce population weight permanently. Additional research using better survey data and stronger study designs is needed to ascertain the long-term effectiveness of an SSB tax on obesity prevalence in MICs
Are Medical Treatments for Individuals and Groups Like Single-Play and Multiple-Play Gambles?
People are often more likely to accept risky gambles with positive expected values when the
gambles will be played more than once. We investigated whether this distinction between singleplay
and multiple-play gambles extends to medical treatments for individual patients and groups
of patients. Resident physicians and medical students (n = 69) and undergraduates (n = 99)
ranked 9 different flu shots and a no-flu-shot option in 1 of 4 combinations of perspective
(individual patient vs. group of 1000 patients) and uncertainty frame (probability vs. frequency).
The rank of the no-flu-shot option (a measure of preference for treatment vs. no treatment) was
not significantly related to perspective or participant population. The main effect of uncertainty
frame and the interaction between perspective and uncertainty frame approached significance
(0.1 < p < 0.05), with the no-flu-shot option faring worst (treatment faring best) when decisions
about many patients were based on frequency information. Undergraduate participants believed
that the no-flu-shot option would be less attractive (treatment would be more attractive) in
decisions about many patients, but these intuitions were inconsistent with the actual ranks. These
results and those of other studies suggest that medical treatments for individuals and groups are
not analogous to single-play and multiple-play monetary gambles, perhaps because people are
unwilling to aggregate treatment outcomes over multiple patients
The Wellcome Trust
The authors conducted a large-scale survey about health care twice, once as a web and once as a random digit dialing (RDD) phone survey. The web survey used a statistical technique, propensity scoring, to adjust for selection bias. Comparing the weighted responses from both surveys, there were no significant response differences in 8 of 37 questions. Web survey responses were significantly more likely to agree with RDD responses when the question asked about the respondent’s personal health (9 times more likely), was a factual question (9 times more likely), and only had two as opposed to multiple AUTHORS’NOTE: This study was funded by the California HealthCare Foundation. We are very grateful for the Foundation’s support. The authors would like to thank George Terhanian and Michael Bosnjak for helpful suggestions on an earlier draft as well as the anonymous referees for very helpful comments