50 research outputs found

    Simulator-Based Driving with Hemianopia: Detection Performance and Compensatory Behaviors on Approach to Intersections

    Get PDF
    OBJECTIVES In 22 states people with homonymous hemianopia (complete loss of the visual field on the sameside in both eyes) are explicitly prohibited from driving, as they do not meet the minimum visualfield requirements for driver licensing. However, there is little scientific evidence derived eitherfrom on-road or driving simulator studies about the safety of driving with hemianopia. If the eyeand head were kept stationary, people with hemianopia would not detect anything on the side ofthe field loss. In the real world, however, they may be able to compensate for the loss byexploring the affected (blind) side using head- and eye-scanning. It has been reported that inHolland (where driving with hemianopia is permitted), driving examiners consider increasedhead-scanning (especially on approach to intersections) to be an effective compensation forperipheral visual field defects (Coeckelbergh et al., 2002). Whether increased head-scanningwhile driving results in better detection performance has never been quantitatively investigated.We conducted a simulator-based evaluation of driving with hemianopia to investigate detectionperformance and head movement behaviors on approach to intersections.METHODSTo date, eight people with complete homonymous hemianopia (5 left and 3 right), and withoutvisual neglect or significant cognitive decline have completed the study. All had current or recentdriving experience (within the last 6 years). They completed two simulator sessions, one weekapart, driving in a high-fidelity simulator. Each session consisted of a familiarization period of30-60 minutes followed by 6 test drives (each about 12 minutes in duration). The primarysimulator task was to detect and respond (by a horn press) to the appearance of pedestrian targetsin a variety of traffic situations while driving according to the normal rules of the road. Targetsappeared randomly in locations relevant to real-world driving. There were two types of targets:“roadway” targets, which appeared either on the left or right of the road at small (~ 4°) or large(~14°) eccentricities from the presumed line of sight, and “intersection” targets, which wereplaced near or at intersections to test whether drivers were scanning effectively whenapproaching an intersection. Primary outcome measures were the percentage of targets detectedand reaction times when detected. Head movements were recorded with an inexpensive, lightweight,head-mounted optical head tracking system. Preliminary analyses of head movementbehaviors were conducted for intersections with stop or yield signs. Based on visual inspectionof the head movement plots, the number and direction of head movements were recorded and head movement scanning was graded on a 4-point scale (from 1 inadequate to 4 excellent). Inaddition, we are developing methods to automatically quantify driving skills (e.g., steering, laneposition) from the simulator data output.RESULTSDetection rates for roadway pedestrian targets were lower and reaction times longer on the blindside than the seeing side (p ≤ 0.05). Blind side: median detection rate 47% (IQR 22 to 63%),median reaction time 1.65s (IQR 1.05 to 1.84s); seeing side: median detection rate 93% (IQR89% to 99%), median reaction time 0.93s, (IQR 0.88 to 1.25s). Detection rates on the blind sidewere lower at the larger eccentricity (median 23%) than the smaller eccentricity (median 66%; p= 0.01). Drivers with right hemianopia (RH) detected 83% of intersection pedestrian targets onthe extreme left of an intersection but none on the extreme right, whereas drivers with lefthemianopia (LH) detected 33% on the extreme left and 80% on the extreme right. Better headscanningscores were associated with better detection rates for intersection targets at extremepositions on the blind side (Spearman r = 0.79, p = 0.02). Two of the drivers with LH showedinadequate scanning (grade 1), failing to scan to the left at more than 60% of intersections. Therest of the drivers with LH and all three with RH demonstrated better head-scanning (grades 2-4)with some compensatory head movement behaviors. At T-intersections with no incoming roadon one side, they scanned more frequently in the direction of the “absent” road when it was onthe blind side (RH 40% and LH 80%) than when it was on the seeing side (RH and LH \u3c10%).When there were incoming roads on both sides, the first head scan was normally to the left forLH, but it was to the right about 30% of the time for drivers with RH.CONCLUSIONSThese results provide evidence of widely varying levels of compensation and detection abilitiesamongst drivers with hemianopia, suggesting that fitness to drive should be evaluated on anindividual basis. The preliminary finding of a relationship between head-scanning score andintersection detection performance will be further evaluated using automated methods toquantify head movement behaviors and a larger sample of drivers with hemianopia. Furthermore,we will compare head movement behaviors of drivers with hemianopia to matched controldrivers without visual field loss.REFERENCESCoeckelbergh, T.R., Brouwer, W.H., Cornelissen, F.W., van Wolffelaar, P., Kooijman, A.C.(2002). The effect of visual field defects on driving performance: a driving simulator study. ArchOphthalmol, 120, 1509-1516

    Measurement of photon sorting at microwave frequencies in a cavity array metasurface

    Get PDF
    PublishedJournal ArticleWe present experimental results demonstrating the spatial sorting of incoming radiation in two spectral ranges. A metasurface composed of a periodically patterned metal of subwavelength thickness with dielectric inclusions concentrates and localizes electromagnetic fields near the surface. Light of the separate spectral bands is channeled into different geometrically tuned cavities within each spatially repeating unit cell. Excitation of cavity modes facilitates this simultaneous spatial- and spectral-selective absorption. The measured reflection and field profiles are presented and the spectral and spatial selectivity are shown. A method to apply these concepts to split radiation into three spectral bands is also proposed.This work was supported in part by the AFOSR Bioenergy project (FA9550-10-1-0350), in part by the NSF Industry/University Cooperative Research Center for Metamaterials (IIP-1068028), and in part by the EPSRC, U.K. funding through the QUEST project (ref: EP/I034548/1)

    A poet's work: Readings and comments

    No full text
    Item consists of a digitized copy of an audio recording of a Vancouver Institute lecture given by Eli Mandel on February 9, 1980. Original audio recording available in the University Archives (UBC AT 908).Other UBCUnreviewedOthe

    Collecting Facts: Between Poetry and the Literature of Fact, 1934–1941

    No full text
    In a time of crisis, does the world need poems or facts? Collecting Facts: Between Poetry and the Literature of Fact, 1934–1941 is a study of four poets, two British and two American, who turned in the 1930s from writing poetry to assembling archives of facts. During a tumultuous decade in which documentary film, photography, and writing flourished, these poets devoted themselves to testing the conceptual limits of facts. For three of these subjects, the ensuing projects were of such size and scope that their authors never completed them to their own satisfaction: Charles Madge’s collaboration with Tom Harrisson and Humphrey Jennings, the organization Mass-Observation (established in 1936); Charles Reznikoff’s Testimony (1934, resumed in 1965); and James Agee’s collaboration with Walker Evans, Let Us Now Praise Famous Men (1941). The fourth poet, W.H. Auden, instead renounced his experiments altogether and returned to a poetry firmly opposed to fact in Another Time (1940). Collecting Facts argues that all four poets tried to create new forms of factual literature that, amid the rise of fascism and the collapse of democratic norms, would represent the world more accurately than existing poetic or documentary modes might be able to do. The first three case studies lead to incompleteness and endless proliferation. In the fourth case, the result is a retreat to conventional forms that, at least on the surface, mark a more narrow conception of what literature might do in the world. All four poets, in other words, are defeated by facts. But in defeat, they offer an account of the role of imaginative literature, and of poetry in particular, over and against factual representation

    Driving with Hemianopia, I: Detection Performance in a Driving Simulator. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science

    No full text
    PURPOSE. This study was designed to examine the effect of homonymous hemianopia (HH) on detection of pedestrian figures in multiple realistic and hazardous situations within the controlled environment of a driving simulator. METHODS. Twelve people with complete HH and without visual neglect or cognitive decline and 12 matched (age, sex, and years of driving experience) normally sighted (NV) drivers participated. They drove predetermined city and rural highway routes (total, 120 minutes) during which pedestrian figures appeared at random intervals along the roadway (R-Peds; n Ď­ 144) and at intersections (I-Peds; n Ď­ 10). Detection rates and response times were derived from participant horn presses. RESULTS. Drivers with HH exhibited significantly (P Ď˝ 0.001) lower R-Ped detection rates on the blind side than did NV drivers (range, 6%-100%). Detection of I-Peds on the blind side was also poor (8%-55%). Age and blind-side detection rates correlated negatively (Spearman r Ď­ ĎŞ0.71, P Ď­ 0.009). Although blind-side response times of drivers with HH were significantly (P Ď˝ 0.001) longer than those of NV drivers, most were within a commonly used 2.5-second guideline. CONCLUSIONS. Most participants with HH had blind-side detection rates that seem incompatible with safe driving; however, the relationship of our simulator detection performance measures to on-road performance has yet to be established. In determining fitness to drive for people with HH, the results underscore the importance of individualized assessments including evaluations of blind-side hazard detection. (Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50:5137-5147

    Reply

    No full text

    The signal-averaged electrocardiogram as a screening test for inducibility of sustained ventricular tachycardia in high risk patients: A prospective study

    Get PDF
    The role of the signal-averaged electrocardiogram in predicting the induction of sustained monomorphic ventricular tachycardia in high risk patients was assessed prospectively in 100 consecutive patients. Presenting diagnoses were syncope (38 patients), nonsustained ventricular tachycardia (24 patients), sustained ventricular tachycardia (25 patients) and sudden cardiac arrest (13 patients). Using programmed ventricular stimulation, 71 patients (group I) did not have and 29 patients (group II) did have inducible sustained monomorphic ventricular tachycardia. Using the signal-averaged electrocardiogram with filtering (6 dB/octave) at high pass corner frequencies of 67 and 100 Hz, the two groups were compared.The signal-averaged electrocardiogram was considered abnormal if allof the following criteria were satisfied: 1) the total filtered QRS complex duration was 120 ms, 2) the duration of the terminal QRS complex of ≤ 20 μV was ≥30 ms, and 3) at least one deflection (late potential) was present in this region. Differences between groups I and II in these three measures were highly significant (p < 0.001). The sensitivity and specificity of signal averaging for predicting the induction of sustained ventricular tachycardia were 93 and 94%, respectively. Stepwise logistic regression analysis identified the signal-averaged electrocardiogram as the best predictor of induction of sustained monomorphic ventricular tachycardia, independent of left ventricular ejection fraction, presence of ventricular aneurysm, myocardial infarction and other clinical variables (chisquare = 93.2, p < 0.0001). The signal-averaged electrocardiogram is a sensitive and specific test for the induction of sustained monomorphic ventricular tachycardia, having independent predictive value
    corecore